Forward telescoping bias in reported age of onset: an example from cigarette smoking

Age at the onset of a disorder is one of its key descriptive characteristics. Early onset may indicate increased risk of a severe course and increased genetic liability. However, retrospectively reported onset in surveys is subject to forward telescoping, a bias in which respondents report events closer to the time of interview than is true. We examined the effect of this bias on age of onset for smoking initiation and daily smoking. Data came from the 1966–77 birth cohorts interviewed in the cross‐sectional National Household Surveys on Drug Abuse (NHDSA) 1979–98 (N = 82,122). An association between age at onset and age at interview, within birth year, was found for experimenters and for daily smokers. This was indicative of forward telescoping. As age at interview increased from 12 to 25 the probability of reporting early onset dropped by half. An association was also found between early onset of daily smoking and age at interview. This response bias differed significantly by sex and race, created significant misclassification of smokers as late instead of early onset cases, and biased cohort comparisons from cross‐sectional data.

[1]  N. Sherwood,et al.  Age of initiation, smoking patterns, and risk in a population of working adults. , 1999, Preventive medicine.

[2]  Robert C. Cloninger,et al.  Bipolar disorder. II: personality and age of onset. , 2003, Bipolar disorders.

[3]  B. Maher,et al.  Genetic linkage of region containing the CREB1 gene to depressive disorders in women from families with recurrent, early-onset, major depression. , 2002, American journal of medical genetics.

[4]  D. Hosmer,et al.  Model‐Building Strategies and Methods for Logistic Regression , 2005 .

[5]  J. Anthony,et al.  Tobacco smoking and depressed mood in late childhood and early adolescence. , 1999, American journal of public health.

[6]  William G. Axinn,et al.  Innovations in life history calendar applications , 1999 .

[7]  C. O'muircheartaigh,et al.  Telescoping of landmark events: implications for survey research. , 2000, Public opinion quarterly.

[8]  D. Kandel,et al.  Stages of progression in drug involvement from adolescence to adulthood: further evidence for the gateway theory. , 1992, Journal of studies on alcohol.

[9]  P. Royston,et al.  Regression using fractional polynomials of continuous covariates: parsimonious parametric modelling. , 1994 .

[10]  D. Kandel,et al.  Patterns of drug use from adolescence to young adulthood: III. Predictors of progression. , 1984, American journal of public health.

[11]  W. Maier,et al.  Test–retest reliability of self‐reported age at onset of selected psychiatric diagnoses in general health care , 2002, Acta psychiatrica Scandinavica.

[12]  K. Raphael,et al.  When did your pain start?: reliability of self-reported age of onset of facial pain. , 1997, The Clinical journal of pain.

[13]  K. Brady,et al.  Cigarette smoking and psychiatric comorbidity in children and adolescents. , 2002, Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry.

[14]  D. Kandel Stages in adolescent involvement in drug use , 1975, Science.

[15]  R. Kessler,et al.  Nicotine dependence in the United States: prevalence, trends, and smoking persistence. , 2001, Archives of general psychiatry.

[16]  H. Wittchen,et al.  Recall and dating of psychiatric symptoms. Test-retest reliability of time-related symptom questions in a standardized psychiatric interview. , 1989, Archives of general psychiatry.

[17]  F P Stafford,et al.  Event history calendars and question list surveys: a direct comparison of interviewing methods. , 2001, Public opinion quarterly.

[18]  L. Thal,et al.  Age at onset is associated with disease severity in Lewy body variant and Alzheimer's disease , 2002, Neuroreport.

[19]  M. Weissman,et al.  Reliability of self-reported age at onset of major depression. , 1989, Journal of psychiatric research.

[20]  K. Rasinski,et al.  Adjusting Survey Estimates for Response Bias: An Application to Trends in Alcohol and Marijuana Use , 1998 .

[21]  N. Brown,et al.  Forward telescoping: the question matters. , 1998, Memory.

[22]  M. Conway,et al.  What happens if you retest autobiographical memory 10 years on? , 2001, Memory & cognition.

[23]  Tx Station Stata Statistical Software: Release 7. , 2001 .

[24]  Bruce D. Johnson,et al.  On Correcting Biases in Self-Reports of Age at First Substance Use with Repeated Cross-Section Analysis , 2000, Journal of quantitative criminology.

[25]  E. Silverman,et al.  Genome-wide linkage analysis of bronchodilator responsiveness and post-bronchodilator spirometric phenotypes in chronic obstructive pulmonary disease. , 2003, Human molecular genetics.

[26]  N. Brown,et al.  The role of guessing and boundaries on date estimation biases , 2004, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[27]  N. Breslau,et al.  Smoking cessation in young adults: age at initiation of cigarette smoking and other suspected influences. , 1996, American journal of public health.

[28]  J. Kaprio,et al.  Genetic liability of type 1 diabetes and the onset age among 22,650 young Finnish twin pairs: a nationwide follow-up study. , 2003, Diabetes.