Individual and ecological determinants of social information transmission in the wild

Social information, acquired through the observation of others, has been documented in a variety of adaptive contexts. The transmission of social information relies on social connections and therefore it is important to consider that individuals may vary in their access to, and use of, such information. Social network analysis allows for the consideration of individual variation in social connections, which until recently has been ignored in the study of social processes. Furthermore, few previous studies of social information use have considered the potential effects of traits such as dominance and personality, which have been found to influence group social structure. We used network-based diffusion analysis, which incorporates information on individual social associations, to examine whether wild flocks of black-capped chickadees, Poecile atricapillus , utilize social information when locating novel foraging patches. Additionally, we incorporated individual traits (age, sex, dominance and exploratory personality) while examining flocks from rural and urban environments, to assess the influence of individual and habitat level characteristics on the rate of information transmission. Social information transmission was found to occur in all flocks, as individual time of discovery of the novel foraging patches was explained by network connections as predicted. However, the only individual level variable found to influence social transmission was dominance rank: dominant individuals had higher rates of information transmission than subordinates. We also found that the rate of social information transmission was higher in rural than urban environments. Our results highlight the importance of considering social associations when examining social information use. Additionally, our results suggest that dominant individuals have greater access to social information than more subordinate individuals, which may demonstrate a previously undocumented additional benefit provided by social dominance.

[1]  W. Hoppitt,et al.  Social Network Analysis Shows Direct Evidence for Social Transmission of Tool Use in Wild Chimpanzees , 2014, PLoS biology.

[2]  Tina W. Wey,et al.  Social network analysis of animal behaviour: a promising tool for the study of sociality , 2008, Animal Behaviour.

[3]  Charles L Nunn,et al.  Network-based diffusion analysis: a new method for detecting social learning , 2009, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[4]  Mikko Mönkkönen,et al.  Social information use is a process across time, space, and ecology, reaching heterospecifics. , 2007, Ecology.

[5]  Hilde Vervaecke,et al.  Measuring and testing the steepness of dominance hierarchies , 2006, Animal Behaviour.

[6]  R. James,et al.  Environmental effects on social interaction networks and male reproductive behaviour in guppies, Poecilia reticulata , 2011, Animal Behaviour.

[7]  L. Giraldeau,et al.  The effect of dominance hierarchy on the use of alternative foraging tactics: a phenotype-limited producing-scrounging game , 1998, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[8]  H. Vries,et al.  Finding a dominance order most consistent with a linear hierarchy: a new procedure and review , 1998, Animal Behaviour.

[9]  William Hoppitt,et al.  Interspecific social networks promote information transmission in wild songbirds , 2015, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[10]  Kevin J. Gaston,et al.  Avian productivity in urban landscapes: a review and meta‐analysis , 2009 .

[11]  Andrew J. J. MacIntosh,et al.  Monkeys in the Middle: Parasite Transmission through the Social Network of a Wild Primate , 2012, PloS one.

[12]  M. Whittingham,et al.  Personality traits in wild starlings: exploration behavior and environmental sensitivity , 2009 .

[13]  A Cockburn,et al.  Individual personalities predict social behaviour in wild networks of great tits (Parus major). , 2013, Ecology letters.

[14]  Alexander E. G. Lee,et al.  Information use and resource competition: an integrative framework , 2016, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[15]  L. Giraldeau,et al.  Chapter 2 Social Foraging and the Study of Exploitative Behavior , 2008 .

[16]  Richard James,et al.  Animal Social Networks , 2014 .

[17]  Zoltán Barta,et al.  THE EFFECTS OF DOMINANCE ON SOCIAL FORAGING TACTIC USE IN HOUSE SPARROWS , 2002 .

[18]  M. Newman,et al.  Identifying the role that animals play in their social networks , 2004, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[19]  L. Giraldeau,et al.  Social influences on foraging in vertebrates: causal mechanisms and adaptive functions , 2001, Animal Behaviour.

[20]  K. Laland Social learning strategies , 2004, Learning & behavior.

[21]  R Core Team,et al.  R: A language and environment for statistical computing. , 2014 .

[22]  P. J. Park,et al.  Correlation between exploration activity and use of social information in three-spined sticklebacks , 2009 .

[23]  K. Laland,et al.  Social Learning: An Introduction to Mechanisms, Methods, and Models , 2013 .

[24]  Aurore Malapert,et al.  Experimental evidence for social transmission of food acquisition techniques in wild meerkats , 2009, Animal Behaviour.

[25]  D. Franks,et al.  Sampling animal association networks with the gambit of the group , 2009, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[26]  Ilya R. Fischhoff,et al.  Network metrics reveal differences in social organization between two fission–fusion species, Grevy’s zebra and onager , 2007, Oecologia.

[27]  A. Thornton,et al.  Experimentally induced innovations lead to persistent culture via conformity in wild birds , 2014, Nature.

[28]  K. Laland,et al.  Information flow through threespine stickleback networks without social transmission , 2012, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[29]  Luke Rendell,et al.  Network-Based Diffusion Analysis Reveals Cultural Transmission of Lobtail Feeding in Humpback Whales , 2013, Science.

[30]  K. E. Bonnie,et al.  Expanding the scope for social information use , 2007, Animal Behaviour.

[31]  Sean F. Hanser,et al.  Social network theory: new insights and issues for behavioral ecologists , 2009, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[32]  Jan Lindström,et al.  Behavioural phenotype affects social interactions in an animal network , 2008, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[33]  Rufus A Johnstone,et al.  Managing uncertainty: information and insurance under the risk of starvation. , 2002, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[34]  A. Rodewald,et al.  The Value of Urban Forests to Wintering Birds , 2006 .

[35]  Richard James,et al.  Behavioural trait assortment in a social network: patterns and implications , 2009, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[36]  B. Kempenaers,et al.  A simple cage test captures intrinsic differences in aspects of personality across individuals in a passerine bird , 2012, Animal Behaviour.

[37]  The economics of tracking a changing environment: competition and social information , 2008, Animal Behaviour.

[38]  Tara S. Stoinski,et al.  Evidence for social learning in wild lemurs (Lemur catta) , 2010, Learning & behavior.

[39]  B. Galef,et al.  Chapter 4 Strategies for Social Learning , 2009 .

[40]  K. Laland,et al.  Environmental Complexity Influences Association Network Structure and Network-Based Diffusion of Foraging Information in Fish Shoals , 2013, The American Naturalist.

[41]  C. Nunn,et al.  Investigating the impact of observation errors on the statistical performance of network-based diffusion analysis , 2010, Learning & behavior.

[42]  Han de Vries,et al.  David's score: a more appropriate dominance ranking method than Clutton-Brock et al.'s index , 2003, Animal Behaviour.

[43]  Kevin N Laland,et al.  Detecting social transmission in networks. , 2010, Journal of theoretical biology.

[44]  P. R. Wiepkema,et al.  Consistent individual differences in early exploratory behaviour of male great tits , 1994, Animal Behaviour.

[45]  Roger Schürch,et al.  The building-up of social relationships: behavioural types, social networks and cooperative breeding in a cichlid , 2010, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[46]  Jennifer J. Templeton,et al.  Environmental Unpredictability and the Value of Social Information for Foraging Starlings , 2003 .

[47]  J. Krause,et al.  Exploring Animal Social Networks , 2008 .

[48]  Sasha R. X. Dall,et al.  Information and its use by animals in evolutionary ecology. , 2005, Trends in ecology & evolution.

[49]  Damien R. Farine,et al.  Animal social network inference and permutations for ecologists in R using asnipe , 2013 .

[50]  D. Mennill,et al.  Social dominance and fitness in black-capped chickadees , 2007 .

[51]  T. Ryder,et al.  Decay of interspecific avian flock networks along a disturbance gradient in Amazonia , 2014, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[52]  Steven J. Schwager,et al.  A comparison of association indices , 1987, Animal Behaviour.

[53]  Glenna F. Nightingale,et al.  Perching but not foraging networks predict the spread of novel foraging skills in starlings , 2014, Behavioural Processes.

[54]  K. Herborn,et al.  Personality in captivity reflects personality in the wild , 2010, Animal Behaviour.

[55]  K. van Oers,et al.  Personality predicts the use of social information. , 2010, Ecology letters.

[56]  Maxime Cauchoix,et al.  Personality does not predict social dominance in wild groups of black-capped chickadees , 2016, Animal Behaviour.

[57]  Lars Chittka,et al.  Speed-accuracy tradeoffs in animal decision making. , 2009, Trends in ecology & evolution.

[58]  Erik Matthysen,et al.  Personality predicts spatial responses to food manipulations in free-ranging great tits (Parus major) , 2010, Biology Letters.

[59]  I. Coolen,et al.  Trade‐Offs in the Adaptive Use of Social and Asocial Learning , 2005 .

[60]  F. Vézina,et al.  Dominant black-capped chickadees pay no maintenance energy costs for their wintering status and are not better at enduring cold than subordinate individuals , 2011, Journal of Comparative Physiology B.

[61]  D. Farine,et al.  Social networks predict patch discovery in a wild population of songbirds , 2012, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[62]  Olivier Duriez,et al.  Impact of food predictability on social facilitation by foraging scavengers , 2010 .

[63]  Kenichi Aoki,et al.  Evolution of learning strategies in temporally and spatially variable environments: a review of theory. , 2014, Theoretical population biology.

[64]  Kees van Oers,et al.  Realized heritability of personalities in the great tit (Parus major) , 2003, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.