A model-based quantification of action control deficits in Parkinson's disease

[1]  R. Ulrich,et al.  On the time-course of automatic response activation in the Simon task , 2018, Psychological research.

[2]  D. Algom,et al.  Stand by Your Stroop: Standing Up Enhances Selective Attention and Cognitive Control , 2017, Psychological science.

[3]  G. Logan,et al.  Neural Bases of Automaticity , 2017, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[4]  C. Li,et al.  Response inhibition in Parkinson’s disease: a meta-analysis of dopaminergic medication and disease duration effects , 2017, npj Parkinson's Disease.

[5]  Borís Burle,et al.  Beyond decision! Motor contribution to speed–accuracy trade-off in decision-making , 2017, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[6]  M. Lee,et al.  New methods, measures, and models for analyzing memory impairment using triadic comparisons , 2016, Behavior research methods.

[7]  H. Leuthold,et al.  Corrigendum to Automatic and controlled stimulus processing in conflict tasks: Superimposed diffusion processes and delta functions [Cogn. Psychol. 78 (2015) 148–174] , 2016, Cognitive Psychology.

[8]  R. Ratcliff,et al.  Anxiety-related threat bias in recognition memory: the moderating effect of list composition and semantic-similarity effects , 2016, Cognition & Emotion.

[9]  Borís Burle,et al.  Linking Theoretical Decision-making Mechanisms in the Simon Task with Electrophysiological Data: A Model-based Neuroscience Study in Humans , 2016, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[10]  Joseph S. Neimat,et al.  Dissociable Effects of Dopamine on the Initial Capture and the Reactive Inhibition of Impulsive Actions in Parkinson's Disease , 2016, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[11]  Scott D. Brown,et al.  Diffusion Decision Model: Current Issues and History , 2016, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[12]  N. Daw,et al.  Dopamine selectively remediates 'model-based' reward learning: a computational approach. , 2016, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[13]  Roger Ratcliff,et al.  A Diffusion Model Analysis of Episodic Recognition in Preclinical Individuals with a Family History for Alzheimer's Disease: the Adult Children Study Diffusion Model Analysis of Episodic Recognition in Preclinical Individuals with a Family , 2022 .

[14]  J. Rothwell,et al.  A fronto–striato–subthalamic–pallidal network for goal-directed and habitual inhibition , 2015, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[15]  Borís Burle,et al.  Using Covert Response Activation to Test Latent Assumptions of Formal Decision-Making Models in Humans , 2015, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[16]  Rolf Ulrich,et al.  Automatic and controlled stimulus processing in conflict tasks: Superimposed diffusion processes and delta functions , 2015, Cognitive Psychology.

[17]  G. Logan,et al.  Inhibitory control in mind and brain 2.0: blocked-input models of saccadic countermanding. , 2015, Psychological review.

[18]  Kristen E. Kanoff,et al.  Speed pressure in conflict situations impedes inhibitory action control in Parkinson's disease , 2014, Biological Psychology.

[19]  Andrew Heathcote,et al.  Brain and Behavior in Decision-Making , 2014, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[20]  Borís Burle,et al.  Conflict tasks and the diffusion framework: Insight in model constraints based on psychological laws , 2014, Cognitive Psychology.

[21]  Scott D. Brown,et al.  The hare and the tortoise: emphasizing speed can change the evidence used to make decisions. , 2014, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[22]  Roshan Cools,et al.  Cognitive deficits in Parkinson's disease: A cognitive neuroscience perspective , 2014, Movement disorders : official journal of the Movement Disorder Society.

[23]  Colm G. Connolly,et al.  Functional connectivity of negative emotional processing in adolescent depression. , 2014, Journal of affective disorders.

[24]  C. Adler,et al.  Disease duration and the integrity of the nigrostriatal system in Parkinson's disease. , 2013, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[25]  Bingni W. Brunton,et al.  Rats and Humans Can Optimally Accumulate Evidence for Decision-Making , 2013, Science.

[26]  M. Lee,et al.  Relating Memory to Functional Performance in Normal Aging to Dementia Using Hierarchical Bayesian Cognitive Processing Models , 2013, Alzheimer disease and associated disorders.

[27]  Richard P. Heitz,et al.  Neural Mechanisms of Speed-Accuracy Tradeoff , 2012, Neuron.

[28]  S. Gilman,et al.  Heterogeneity of Cholinergic Denervation in Parkinson's Disease without Dementia , 2012, Journal of cerebral blood flow and metabolism : official journal of the International Society of Cerebral Blood Flow and Metabolism.

[29]  K. Richard Ridderinkhof,et al.  Dopamine Agonists and the Suppression of Impulsive Motor Actions in Parkinson Disease , 2012, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[30]  Wolf Schwarz,et al.  Response time models of delta plots with negative-going slopes , 2012, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.

[31]  Roshan Cools,et al.  Habitual versus Goal-directed Action Control in Parkinson Disease , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[32]  James D. Miles,et al.  Reaction time distribution analysis of spatial correspondence effects , 2011, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[33]  Hartmut Leuthold,et al.  The Simon effect in cognitive electrophysiology: A short review. , 2011, Acta psychologica.

[34]  B. Hommel The Simon effect as tool and heuristic. , 2011, Acta psychologica.

[35]  K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al.  Version unknown SOURCE ( OR PART OF THE FOLLOWING SOURCE ) : Type article Title To head or to heed ? Beyond the surface of selective action inhibition : a review , 2022 .

[36]  H. Bergman,et al.  Goal-directed and habitual control in the basal ganglia: implications for Parkinson's disease , 2010, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[37]  Jeffrey N Rouder,et al.  Exploring the differences in distributional properties between Stroop and Simon effects using delta plots , 2010, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[38]  Roger Ratcliff,et al.  Anxiety enhances threat processing without competition among multiple inputs: a diffusion model analysis. , 2010, Emotion.

[39]  K. Richard Ridderinkhof,et al.  The Effect of Parkinson's Disease on the Dynamics of On-line and Proactive Cognitive Control during Action Selection , 2010, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[40]  R. Ratcliff,et al.  Using diffusion models to understand clinical disorders. , 2010, Journal of mathematical psychology.

[41]  R. Bogacz,et al.  The neural basis of the speed–accuracy tradeoff , 2010, Trends in Neurosciences.

[42]  K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al.  The effect of speed-accuracy strategy on response interference control in Parkinson's disease , 2009, Neuropsychologia.

[43]  Philip L. Smith,et al.  An integrated theory of attention and decision making in visual signal detection. , 2009, Psychological review.

[44]  S. Aalto,et al.  Impaired cognitive performance in Parkinson's disease is related to caudate dopaminergic hypofunction and hippocampal atrophy. , 2009, Parkinsonism & related disorders.

[45]  Daniel Weintraub,et al.  Montreal Cognitive Assessment Performance in Patients with Parkinson's Disease with “Normal” Global Cognition According to Mini‐Mental State Examination Score , 2009, Journal of the American Geriatrics Society.

[46]  K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al.  The effect of Parkinson's disease on interference control during action selection , 2009, Neuropsychologia.

[47]  Roger Ratcliff,et al.  The Diffusion Decision Model: Theory and Data for Two-Choice Decision Tasks , 2008, Neural Computation.

[48]  A. Aron The Neural Basis of Inhibition in Cognitive Control , 2007, The Neuroscientist : a review journal bringing neurobiology, neurology and psychiatry.

[49]  G. Logan,et al.  Inhibitory control in mind and brain: an interactive race model of countermanding saccades. , 2007, Psychological review.

[50]  Christopher R. Myers,et al.  Universally Sloppy Parameter Sensitivities in Systems Biology Models , 2007, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[51]  K. Jellinger The morphological basis of mental dysfunction in Parkinson's disease , 2006, Journal of the Neurological Sciences.

[52]  Michael J. Frank,et al.  Hold your horses: A dynamic computational role for the subthalamic nucleus in decision making , 2006, Neural Networks.

[53]  H. Yin,et al.  The role of the basal ganglia in habit formation , 2006, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[54]  J. Stout,et al.  Activation of conflicting responses in Parkinson's disease: evidence for degrading and facilitating effects on response time , 2005, Neuropsychologia.

[55]  G. Pari,et al.  Deficits in saccadic eye-movement control in Parkinson's disease , 2005, Neuropsychologia.

[56]  J. Cummings,et al.  The Montreal Cognitive Assessment, MoCA: A Brief Screening Tool For Mild Cognitive Impairment , 2005, Journal of the American Geriatrics Society.

[57]  Michael J. Frank,et al.  By Carrot or by Stick: Cognitive Reinforcement Learning in Parkinsonism , 2004, Science.

[58]  Manuel Perea,et al.  A diffusion model account of normal and impaired readers , 2004, Brain and Cognition.

[59]  Philip L. Smith,et al.  A comparison of sequential sampling models for two-choice reaction time. , 2004, Psychological review.

[60]  R. Ratcliff,et al.  Estimating parameters of the diffusion model: Approaches to dealing with contaminant reaction times and parameter variability , 2002, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[61]  A. Brück,et al.  Positron emission tomography shows that impaired frontal lobe functioning in Parkinson’s disease is related to dopaminergic hypofunction in the caudate nucleus , 2001, Neuroscience Letters.

[62]  Peter Praamstra,et al.  Motor cortex activation in Parkinson's disease: Dissociation of electrocortical and peripheral measures of response generation , 1999, Movement disorders : official journal of the Movement Disorder Society.

[63]  Jeffrey N. Rouder,et al.  Modeling Response Times for Two-Choice Decisions , 1998 .

[64]  R. Proctor,et al.  The influence of irrelevant location information on performance: A review of the Simon and spatial Stroop effects , 1995, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[65]  E. Lauber,et al.  Conditional and unconditional automaticity: a dual-process model of effects of spatial stimulus-response correspondence. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[66]  B. Hommel The relationship between stimulus processing and response selection in the Simon task: Evidence for a temporal overlap , 1993 .

[67]  T. Tombaugh,et al.  The Mini‐Mental State Examination: A Comprehensive Review , 1992, Journal of the American Geriatrics Society.

[68]  A. Osman,et al.  Dimensional overlap: cognitive basis for stimulus-response compatibility--a model and taxonomy. , 1990, Psychological review.

[69]  G. Logan Toward an instance theory of automatization. , 1988 .

[70]  C. Eriksen,et al.  Pre- and poststimulus activation of response channels: a psychophysiological analysis. , 1988, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[71]  C. Eriksen,et al.  Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance , 2004 .

[72]  C. Eriksen,et al.  An electromyographic examination of response competition , 1985 .

[73]  G. Logan Attention and automaticity in Stroop and priming tasks: Theory and data , 1980, Cognitive Psychology.

[74]  Roger Ratcliff,et al.  A Theory of Memory Retrieval. , 1978 .

[75]  Walter Schneider,et al.  Controlled and automatic human information processing: II. Perceptual learning, automatic attending and a general theory. , 1977 .

[76]  S. Folstein,et al.  "Mini-mental state". A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. , 1975, Journal of psychiatric research.

[77]  K. Jellinger,et al.  Brain dopamine and the syndromes of Parkinson and Huntington. Clinical, morphological and neurochemical correlations. , 1973, Journal of the neurological sciences.

[78]  J R Simon,et al.  Processing auditory information: interference from an irrelevant cue. , 1969, The Journal of applied psychology.

[79]  M. Hoehn,et al.  Parkinsonism , 1967, Neurology.

[80]  Roger Ratcliff,et al.  Parameter variability and distributional assumptions in the diffusion model. , 2013, Psychological review.

[81]  B. Balleine,et al.  Human and Rodent Homologies in Action Control: Corticostriatal Determinants of Goal-Directed and Habitual Action , 2010, Neuropsychopharmacology.

[82]  R. Poldrack,et al.  Cortical and Subcortical Contributions to Stop Signal Response Inhibition: Role of the Subthalamic Nucleus , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[83]  Michael J. Frank,et al.  Dynamic Dopamine Modulation in the Basal Ganglia: A Neurocomputational Account of Cognitive Deficits in Medicated and Nonmedicated Parkinsonism , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[84]  K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al.  Activation and suppression in conflict tasks: Empirical clarification through distributional analyses. , 2002 .

[85]  P. Praamstra,et al.  Failed Suppression of Direct Visuomotor Activation in Parkinson's Disease , 2001, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[86]  D F Stegeman,et al.  Reliance on external cues for movement initiation in Parkinson's disease. Evidence from movement-related potentials. , 1998, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[87]  B. Hommel Spontaneous decay of response-code activation , 1994, Psychological research.

[88]  J. Richard Simon,et al.  The effect of an irrelevant directional cue on choice reaction time: Duration of the phenomenon and its relation to stages of processing , 1976 .

[89]  C. Eriksen,et al.  Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task , 1974 .

[90]  John A. Nelder,et al.  A Simplex Method for Function Minimization , 1965, Comput. J..