A model-based quantification of action control deficits in Parkinson's disease
暂无分享,去创建一个
Gordon D. Logan | Mathieu Servant | Scott A. Wylie | G. Logan | N. V. Wouwe | Mathieu Servant | S. Wylie | Nelleke van Wouwe
[1] R. Ulrich,et al. On the time-course of automatic response activation in the Simon task , 2018, Psychological research.
[2] D. Algom,et al. Stand by Your Stroop: Standing Up Enhances Selective Attention and Cognitive Control , 2017, Psychological science.
[3] G. Logan,et al. Neural Bases of Automaticity , 2017, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[4] C. Li,et al. Response inhibition in Parkinson’s disease: a meta-analysis of dopaminergic medication and disease duration effects , 2017, npj Parkinson's Disease.
[5] Borís Burle,et al. Beyond decision! Motor contribution to speed–accuracy trade-off in decision-making , 2017, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[6] M. Lee,et al. New methods, measures, and models for analyzing memory impairment using triadic comparisons , 2016, Behavior research methods.
[7] H. Leuthold,et al. Corrigendum to Automatic and controlled stimulus processing in conflict tasks: Superimposed diffusion processes and delta functions [Cogn. Psychol. 78 (2015) 148–174] , 2016, Cognitive Psychology.
[8] R. Ratcliff,et al. Anxiety-related threat bias in recognition memory: the moderating effect of list composition and semantic-similarity effects , 2016, Cognition & Emotion.
[9] Borís Burle,et al. Linking Theoretical Decision-making Mechanisms in the Simon Task with Electrophysiological Data: A Model-based Neuroscience Study in Humans , 2016, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[10] Joseph S. Neimat,et al. Dissociable Effects of Dopamine on the Initial Capture and the Reactive Inhibition of Impulsive Actions in Parkinson's Disease , 2016, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[11] Scott D. Brown,et al. Diffusion Decision Model: Current Issues and History , 2016, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[12] N. Daw,et al. Dopamine selectively remediates 'model-based' reward learning: a computational approach. , 2016, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[13] Roger Ratcliff,et al. A Diffusion Model Analysis of Episodic Recognition in Preclinical Individuals with a Family History for Alzheimer's Disease: the Adult Children Study Diffusion Model Analysis of Episodic Recognition in Preclinical Individuals with a Family , 2022 .
[14] J. Rothwell,et al. A fronto–striato–subthalamic–pallidal network for goal-directed and habitual inhibition , 2015, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[15] Borís Burle,et al. Using Covert Response Activation to Test Latent Assumptions of Formal Decision-Making Models in Humans , 2015, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[16] Rolf Ulrich,et al. Automatic and controlled stimulus processing in conflict tasks: Superimposed diffusion processes and delta functions , 2015, Cognitive Psychology.
[17] G. Logan,et al. Inhibitory control in mind and brain 2.0: blocked-input models of saccadic countermanding. , 2015, Psychological review.
[18] Kristen E. Kanoff,et al. Speed pressure in conflict situations impedes inhibitory action control in Parkinson's disease , 2014, Biological Psychology.
[19] Andrew Heathcote,et al. Brain and Behavior in Decision-Making , 2014, PLoS Comput. Biol..
[20] Borís Burle,et al. Conflict tasks and the diffusion framework: Insight in model constraints based on psychological laws , 2014, Cognitive Psychology.
[21] Scott D. Brown,et al. The hare and the tortoise: emphasizing speed can change the evidence used to make decisions. , 2014, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[22] Roshan Cools,et al. Cognitive deficits in Parkinson's disease: A cognitive neuroscience perspective , 2014, Movement disorders : official journal of the Movement Disorder Society.
[23] Colm G. Connolly,et al. Functional connectivity of negative emotional processing in adolescent depression. , 2014, Journal of affective disorders.
[24] C. Adler,et al. Disease duration and the integrity of the nigrostriatal system in Parkinson's disease. , 2013, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[25] Bingni W. Brunton,et al. Rats and Humans Can Optimally Accumulate Evidence for Decision-Making , 2013, Science.
[26] M. Lee,et al. Relating Memory to Functional Performance in Normal Aging to Dementia Using Hierarchical Bayesian Cognitive Processing Models , 2013, Alzheimer disease and associated disorders.
[27] Richard P. Heitz,et al. Neural Mechanisms of Speed-Accuracy Tradeoff , 2012, Neuron.
[28] S. Gilman,et al. Heterogeneity of Cholinergic Denervation in Parkinson's Disease without Dementia , 2012, Journal of cerebral blood flow and metabolism : official journal of the International Society of Cerebral Blood Flow and Metabolism.
[29] K. Richard Ridderinkhof,et al. Dopamine Agonists and the Suppression of Impulsive Motor Actions in Parkinson Disease , 2012, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[30] Wolf Schwarz,et al. Response time models of delta plots with negative-going slopes , 2012, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.
[31] Roshan Cools,et al. Habitual versus Goal-directed Action Control in Parkinson Disease , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[32] James D. Miles,et al. Reaction time distribution analysis of spatial correspondence effects , 2011, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[33] Hartmut Leuthold,et al. The Simon effect in cognitive electrophysiology: A short review. , 2011, Acta psychologica.
[34] B. Hommel. The Simon effect as tool and heuristic. , 2011, Acta psychologica.
[35] K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al. Version unknown SOURCE ( OR PART OF THE FOLLOWING SOURCE ) : Type article Title To head or to heed ? Beyond the surface of selective action inhibition : a review , 2022 .
[36] H. Bergman,et al. Goal-directed and habitual control in the basal ganglia: implications for Parkinson's disease , 2010, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[37] Jeffrey N Rouder,et al. Exploring the differences in distributional properties between Stroop and Simon effects using delta plots , 2010, Attention, perception & psychophysics.
[38] Roger Ratcliff,et al. Anxiety enhances threat processing without competition among multiple inputs: a diffusion model analysis. , 2010, Emotion.
[39] K. Richard Ridderinkhof,et al. The Effect of Parkinson's Disease on the Dynamics of On-line and Proactive Cognitive Control during Action Selection , 2010, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[40] R. Ratcliff,et al. Using diffusion models to understand clinical disorders. , 2010, Journal of mathematical psychology.
[41] R. Bogacz,et al. The neural basis of the speed–accuracy tradeoff , 2010, Trends in Neurosciences.
[42] K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al. The effect of speed-accuracy strategy on response interference control in Parkinson's disease , 2009, Neuropsychologia.
[43] Philip L. Smith,et al. An integrated theory of attention and decision making in visual signal detection. , 2009, Psychological review.
[44] S. Aalto,et al. Impaired cognitive performance in Parkinson's disease is related to caudate dopaminergic hypofunction and hippocampal atrophy. , 2009, Parkinsonism & related disorders.
[45] Daniel Weintraub,et al. Montreal Cognitive Assessment Performance in Patients with Parkinson's Disease with “Normal” Global Cognition According to Mini‐Mental State Examination Score , 2009, Journal of the American Geriatrics Society.
[46] K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al. The effect of Parkinson's disease on interference control during action selection , 2009, Neuropsychologia.
[47] Roger Ratcliff,et al. The Diffusion Decision Model: Theory and Data for Two-Choice Decision Tasks , 2008, Neural Computation.
[48] A. Aron. The Neural Basis of Inhibition in Cognitive Control , 2007, The Neuroscientist : a review journal bringing neurobiology, neurology and psychiatry.
[49] G. Logan,et al. Inhibitory control in mind and brain: an interactive race model of countermanding saccades. , 2007, Psychological review.
[50] Christopher R. Myers,et al. Universally Sloppy Parameter Sensitivities in Systems Biology Models , 2007, PLoS Comput. Biol..
[51] K. Jellinger. The morphological basis of mental dysfunction in Parkinson's disease , 2006, Journal of the Neurological Sciences.
[52] Michael J. Frank,et al. Hold your horses: A dynamic computational role for the subthalamic nucleus in decision making , 2006, Neural Networks.
[53] H. Yin,et al. The role of the basal ganglia in habit formation , 2006, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[54] J. Stout,et al. Activation of conflicting responses in Parkinson's disease: evidence for degrading and facilitating effects on response time , 2005, Neuropsychologia.
[55] G. Pari,et al. Deficits in saccadic eye-movement control in Parkinson's disease , 2005, Neuropsychologia.
[56] J. Cummings,et al. The Montreal Cognitive Assessment, MoCA: A Brief Screening Tool For Mild Cognitive Impairment , 2005, Journal of the American Geriatrics Society.
[57] Michael J. Frank,et al. By Carrot or by Stick: Cognitive Reinforcement Learning in Parkinsonism , 2004, Science.
[58] Manuel Perea,et al. A diffusion model account of normal and impaired readers , 2004, Brain and Cognition.
[59] Philip L. Smith,et al. A comparison of sequential sampling models for two-choice reaction time. , 2004, Psychological review.
[60] R. Ratcliff,et al. Estimating parameters of the diffusion model: Approaches to dealing with contaminant reaction times and parameter variability , 2002, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[61] A. Brück,et al. Positron emission tomography shows that impaired frontal lobe functioning in Parkinson’s disease is related to dopaminergic hypofunction in the caudate nucleus , 2001, Neuroscience Letters.
[62] Peter Praamstra,et al. Motor cortex activation in Parkinson's disease: Dissociation of electrocortical and peripheral measures of response generation , 1999, Movement disorders : official journal of the Movement Disorder Society.
[63] Jeffrey N. Rouder,et al. Modeling Response Times for Two-Choice Decisions , 1998 .
[64] R. Proctor,et al. The influence of irrelevant location information on performance: A review of the Simon and spatial Stroop effects , 1995, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[65] E. Lauber,et al. Conditional and unconditional automaticity: a dual-process model of effects of spatial stimulus-response correspondence. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[66] B. Hommel. The relationship between stimulus processing and response selection in the Simon task: Evidence for a temporal overlap , 1993 .
[67] T. Tombaugh,et al. The Mini‐Mental State Examination: A Comprehensive Review , 1992, Journal of the American Geriatrics Society.
[68] A. Osman,et al. Dimensional overlap: cognitive basis for stimulus-response compatibility--a model and taxonomy. , 1990, Psychological review.
[69] G. Logan. Toward an instance theory of automatization. , 1988 .
[70] C. Eriksen,et al. Pre- and poststimulus activation of response channels: a psychophysiological analysis. , 1988, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[71] C. Eriksen,et al. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance , 2004 .
[72] C. Eriksen,et al. An electromyographic examination of response competition , 1985 .
[73] G. Logan. Attention and automaticity in Stroop and priming tasks: Theory and data , 1980, Cognitive Psychology.
[74] Roger Ratcliff,et al. A Theory of Memory Retrieval. , 1978 .
[75] Walter Schneider,et al. Controlled and automatic human information processing: II. Perceptual learning, automatic attending and a general theory. , 1977 .
[76] S. Folstein,et al. "Mini-mental state". A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. , 1975, Journal of psychiatric research.
[77] K. Jellinger,et al. Brain dopamine and the syndromes of Parkinson and Huntington. Clinical, morphological and neurochemical correlations. , 1973, Journal of the neurological sciences.
[78] J R Simon,et al. Processing auditory information: interference from an irrelevant cue. , 1969, The Journal of applied psychology.
[79] M. Hoehn,et al. Parkinsonism , 1967, Neurology.
[80] Roger Ratcliff,et al. Parameter variability and distributional assumptions in the diffusion model. , 2013, Psychological review.
[81] B. Balleine,et al. Human and Rodent Homologies in Action Control: Corticostriatal Determinants of Goal-Directed and Habitual Action , 2010, Neuropsychopharmacology.
[82] R. Poldrack,et al. Cortical and Subcortical Contributions to Stop Signal Response Inhibition: Role of the Subthalamic Nucleus , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[83] Michael J. Frank,et al. Dynamic Dopamine Modulation in the Basal Ganglia: A Neurocomputational Account of Cognitive Deficits in Medicated and Nonmedicated Parkinsonism , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[84] K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al. Activation and suppression in conflict tasks: Empirical clarification through distributional analyses. , 2002 .
[85] P. Praamstra,et al. Failed Suppression of Direct Visuomotor Activation in Parkinson's Disease , 2001, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[86] D F Stegeman,et al. Reliance on external cues for movement initiation in Parkinson's disease. Evidence from movement-related potentials. , 1998, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[87] B. Hommel. Spontaneous decay of response-code activation , 1994, Psychological research.
[88] J. Richard Simon,et al. The effect of an irrelevant directional cue on choice reaction time: Duration of the phenomenon and its relation to stages of processing , 1976 .
[89] C. Eriksen,et al. Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task , 1974 .
[90] John A. Nelder,et al. A Simplex Method for Function Minimization , 1965, Comput. J..