Lifetime low-level lead exposure produces deficits in delayed alternation in adult monkeys.

[1]  C Waternaux,et al.  Longitudinal analyses of prenatal and postnatal lead exposure and early cognitive development. , 1987, The New England journal of medicine.

[2]  H. Lilienthal,et al.  Pre- and postnatal lead-exposure in monkeys: effects on activity and learning set formation. , 1986, Neurobehavioral toxicology and teratology.

[3]  C. Waternaux,et al.  Low-level lead exposure and infant development in the first year. , 1986, Neurobehavioral toxicology and teratology.

[4]  M. Urbanowicz,et al.  The relationship between blood-lead concentrations, intelligence, attainment and behaviour in a school population: the second London study , 1986, International archives of occupational and environmental health.

[5]  D. Rice,et al.  Low lead exposure from birth produces behavioral toxicity (DRL) in monkeys. , 1985, Toxicology and applied pharmacology.

[6]  D. Rice,et al.  Chronic low-lead exposure from birth produces deficits in discrimination reversal in monkeys. , 1985, Toxicology and applied pharmacology.

[7]  D. Rice Behavioral deficit (delayed matching to sample) in monkeys exposed from birth to low levels of lead. , 1984, Toxicology and applied pharmacology.

[8]  C. Waternaux,et al.  Early sensory-motor development and prenatal exposure to lead. , 1984, Neurobehavioral toxicology and teratology.

[9]  G. Winneke,et al.  Neuropsychological effects of lead in children: interactions with social background variables. , 1984, Neuropsychobiology.

[10]  E. Levin,et al.  The effect of pre- or postnatal lead exposure on Hamilton Search Task in monkeys. , 1983, Neurobehavioral toxicology and teratology.

[11]  W. Janke,et al.  Neuropsychological studies in children with elevated tooth-lead concentrations , 1983, International archives of occupational and environmental health.

[12]  G. Winneke,et al.  Neuropsychological studies in children with elevated tooth-lead concentrations , 1983, International archives of occupational and environmental health.

[13]  J. Annest,et al.  National estimates of blood lead levels: United States, 1976-1980: association with selected demographic and socioeconomic factors. , 1982, The New England journal of medicine.

[14]  P. Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Spatial memory impairments following damage to the mediodorsal nucleus of the thalamus in rhesus monkeys , 1982, Brain Research.

[15]  William Yule,et al.  The Relationship between Blood Lead Concentrations, Intelligence and Attainment in a School Population: a Pilot Study , 1981, Developmental medicine and child neurology.

[16]  C. Thompson Learning in rhesus monkeys after amygdalectomy in infancy or adulthood , 1981, Behavioural Brain Research.

[17]  Philip J. Bushnell,et al.  Persistence of impaired reversal learning in young monkeys exposed to low levels of dietary lead. , 1979, Journal of toxicology and environmental health.

[18]  P. Good,et al.  Detection of a treatment effect when not all experimental subjects will respond to treatment. , 1979, Biometrics.

[19]  P. Bushnell,et al.  Reversal learning deficits in young monkeys exposed to lead , 1979, Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior.

[20]  A. Leviton,et al.  Deficits in psychologic and classroom performance of children with elevated dentine lead levels. , 1979, The New England journal of medicine.

[21]  Rice Dc,et al.  Neonatal low-level lead exposure in monkeys (Macaca fascicularis): effect on two-choice non-spatial form discrimination. , 1979 .

[22]  Steven G. Gilbert,et al.  NOVA SKED II: A behavioral notation language utilizing the Data General Corporation real-time disk operating system , 1979 .

[23]  Herbert L. Needleman,et al.  Preventing lead poisoning in young children , 1978 .

[24]  H. Markowitsch,et al.  Comparative analysis of prefrontal learning functions in rats, cats, and monkeys , 1977 .

[25]  Willes Rf,et al.  Nursery rearing of infant monkeys (Macaca fascicularis) for toxicity studies. , 1977 .

[26]  P. S. Goldman The role of experience in recovery of function following orbital prefrontal lesions in infant monkeys , 1976, Neuropsychologia.

[27]  N. Butters,et al.  Behavioral effects of sequential and one-stage ablations of orbital prefrontal cortex in the monkey. , 1973, Experimental neurology.

[28]  H. E. Rosvold,et al.  The effects of selective caudate lesions in infant and juvenile Rhesus monkeys. , 1972, Brain research.

[29]  D. Pandya,et al.  A search for the spatial engram within the frontal lobes of monkeys. , 1972, Acta neurobiologiae experimentalis.

[30]  H. Mahut Spatial and object reversal learning in monkeys with partial temporal lobe ablations. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[31]  H. E. Rosvold,et al.  Analysis of the delayed-alternation deficit produced by dorsolateral prefrontal lesions in the rhesus monkey. , 1971, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[32]  J. Stamm,et al.  Delayed alternation impairments following selective prefrontal cortical ablations in monkeys. , 1971, Experimental neurology.

[33]  F. R. Treichler,et al.  The influence of delay interval on severity of the spatial alternation deficit in frontal monkeys. , 1971, Cortex; a journal devoted to the study of the nervous system and behavior.

[34]  M. Mishkin,et al.  Evidence for behavioral impairment following prefrontal lobectomy in the infant monkey. , 1970, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[35]  Charles M. Butter,et al.  Perseveration in extinction and in discrimination reversal tasks following selective frontal ablations in Macaca mulatta , 1969 .

[36]  N. Butters,et al.  Effect of septal lesions on resistance to extinction and delayed alternation in monkeys. , 1968, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[37]  R. Potthoff,et al.  A generalized multivariate analysis of variance model useful especially for growth curve problems , 1964 .

[38]  H. Harlow,et al.  Analysis of frontal and posterior association syndromes in brain-damaged monkeys. , 1952, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.