Lifetime low-level lead exposure produces deficits in delayed alternation in adult monkeys.
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] C Waternaux,et al. Longitudinal analyses of prenatal and postnatal lead exposure and early cognitive development. , 1987, The New England journal of medicine.
[2] H. Lilienthal,et al. Pre- and postnatal lead-exposure in monkeys: effects on activity and learning set formation. , 1986, Neurobehavioral toxicology and teratology.
[3] C. Waternaux,et al. Low-level lead exposure and infant development in the first year. , 1986, Neurobehavioral toxicology and teratology.
[4] M. Urbanowicz,et al. The relationship between blood-lead concentrations, intelligence, attainment and behaviour in a school population: the second London study , 1986, International archives of occupational and environmental health.
[5] D. Rice,et al. Low lead exposure from birth produces behavioral toxicity (DRL) in monkeys. , 1985, Toxicology and applied pharmacology.
[6] D. Rice,et al. Chronic low-lead exposure from birth produces deficits in discrimination reversal in monkeys. , 1985, Toxicology and applied pharmacology.
[7] D. Rice. Behavioral deficit (delayed matching to sample) in monkeys exposed from birth to low levels of lead. , 1984, Toxicology and applied pharmacology.
[8] C. Waternaux,et al. Early sensory-motor development and prenatal exposure to lead. , 1984, Neurobehavioral toxicology and teratology.
[9] G. Winneke,et al. Neuropsychological effects of lead in children: interactions with social background variables. , 1984, Neuropsychobiology.
[10] E. Levin,et al. The effect of pre- or postnatal lead exposure on Hamilton Search Task in monkeys. , 1983, Neurobehavioral toxicology and teratology.
[11] W. Janke,et al. Neuropsychological studies in children with elevated tooth-lead concentrations , 1983, International archives of occupational and environmental health.
[12] G. Winneke,et al. Neuropsychological studies in children with elevated tooth-lead concentrations , 1983, International archives of occupational and environmental health.
[13] J. Annest,et al. National estimates of blood lead levels: United States, 1976-1980: association with selected demographic and socioeconomic factors. , 1982, The New England journal of medicine.
[14] P. Goldman-Rakic,et al. Spatial memory impairments following damage to the mediodorsal nucleus of the thalamus in rhesus monkeys , 1982, Brain Research.
[15] William Yule,et al. The Relationship between Blood Lead Concentrations, Intelligence and Attainment in a School Population: a Pilot Study , 1981, Developmental medicine and child neurology.
[16] C. Thompson. Learning in rhesus monkeys after amygdalectomy in infancy or adulthood , 1981, Behavioural Brain Research.
[17] Philip J. Bushnell,et al. Persistence of impaired reversal learning in young monkeys exposed to low levels of dietary lead. , 1979, Journal of toxicology and environmental health.
[18] P. Good,et al. Detection of a treatment effect when not all experimental subjects will respond to treatment. , 1979, Biometrics.
[19] P. Bushnell,et al. Reversal learning deficits in young monkeys exposed to lead , 1979, Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior.
[20] A. Leviton,et al. Deficits in psychologic and classroom performance of children with elevated dentine lead levels. , 1979, The New England journal of medicine.
[21] Rice Dc,et al. Neonatal low-level lead exposure in monkeys (Macaca fascicularis): effect on two-choice non-spatial form discrimination. , 1979 .
[22] Steven G. Gilbert,et al. NOVA SKED II: A behavioral notation language utilizing the Data General Corporation real-time disk operating system , 1979 .
[23] Herbert L. Needleman,et al. Preventing lead poisoning in young children , 1978 .
[24] H. Markowitsch,et al. Comparative analysis of prefrontal learning functions in rats, cats, and monkeys , 1977 .
[25] Willes Rf,et al. Nursery rearing of infant monkeys (Macaca fascicularis) for toxicity studies. , 1977 .
[26] P. S. Goldman. The role of experience in recovery of function following orbital prefrontal lesions in infant monkeys , 1976, Neuropsychologia.
[27] N. Butters,et al. Behavioral effects of sequential and one-stage ablations of orbital prefrontal cortex in the monkey. , 1973, Experimental neurology.
[28] H. E. Rosvold,et al. The effects of selective caudate lesions in infant and juvenile Rhesus monkeys. , 1972, Brain research.
[29] D. Pandya,et al. A search for the spatial engram within the frontal lobes of monkeys. , 1972, Acta neurobiologiae experimentalis.
[30] H. Mahut. Spatial and object reversal learning in monkeys with partial temporal lobe ablations. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.
[31] H. E. Rosvold,et al. Analysis of the delayed-alternation deficit produced by dorsolateral prefrontal lesions in the rhesus monkey. , 1971, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.
[32] J. Stamm,et al. Delayed alternation impairments following selective prefrontal cortical ablations in monkeys. , 1971, Experimental neurology.
[33] F. R. Treichler,et al. The influence of delay interval on severity of the spatial alternation deficit in frontal monkeys. , 1971, Cortex; a journal devoted to the study of the nervous system and behavior.
[34] M. Mishkin,et al. Evidence for behavioral impairment following prefrontal lobectomy in the infant monkey. , 1970, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.
[35] Charles M. Butter,et al. Perseveration in extinction and in discrimination reversal tasks following selective frontal ablations in Macaca mulatta , 1969 .
[36] N. Butters,et al. Effect of septal lesions on resistance to extinction and delayed alternation in monkeys. , 1968, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.
[37] R. Potthoff,et al. A generalized multivariate analysis of variance model useful especially for growth curve problems , 1964 .
[38] H. Harlow,et al. Analysis of frontal and posterior association syndromes in brain-damaged monkeys. , 1952, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.