Fetal Origin of Sensorimotor Behavior

The aim of this article is to track the fetal origin of infants’ sensorimotor behavior. We consider development as the self-organizing emergence of complex forms from spontaneously generated activity, governed by the innate capacity to detect and memorize the consequences of spontaneous activity (contingencies), and constrained by the sensory and motor maturation of the body. In support of this view, we show how observations on fetuses and also several fetal experiments suggest that the fetus’s first motor activity allows it to feel the space around it and to feel its body and the consequences of its movements on its body. This primitive motor babbling gives way progressively to sensorimotor behavior which already possesses most of the characteristics of infants’ later behavior: repetition of actions leading to sensations, intentionality, some motor control and oriented reactions to sensory stimulation. In this way the fetus can start developing a body map and acquiring knowledge of its limited physical and social environment.

[1]  H. G. Marques,et al.  Twitching in Sensorimotor Development from Sleeping Rats to Robots , 2013, Current Biology.

[2]  N. Reissland,et al.  The development of anticipation in the fetus: a longitudinal account of human fetal mouth movements in reaction to and anticipation of touch. , 2014, Developmental psychobiology.

[3]  H. Prechtl Qualitative changes of spontaneous movements in fetus and preterm infant are a marker of neurological dysfunction. , 1990, Early human development.

[4]  P. Ferrari,et al.  Ultrasonographic Investigation of Human Fetus Responses to Maternal Communicative and Non-communicative Stimuli , 2016, Front. Psychol..

[5]  Mark H. Johnson,et al.  Gaze Following in Newborns , 2004 .

[6]  Emese Nagy,et al.  Fetal Behavioural Responses to Maternal Voice and Touch , 2015, PloS one.

[7]  J. Fagard,et al.  The effect of task constraints on infants’ (bi)manual strategy for grasping and exploring objects , 2005 .

[8]  Tim Donovan,et al.  The Human Fetus Preferentially Engages with Face-like Visual Stimuli , 2017, Current Biology.

[9]  R. Held,et al.  OBSERVATIONS ON THE DEVELOPMENT OF VISUALLY-DIRECTED REACHING. , 1964, Child development.

[10]  Linda B. Smith,et al.  Developmental process emerges from extended brain–body–behavior networks , 2014, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[11]  U. Castiello,et al.  Evidence of early development of action planning in the human foetus: a kinematic study , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.

[12]  H B Valman,et al.  What the fetus feels. , 1980, British medical journal.

[13]  M. Hadders‐Algra,et al.  Fetal Onset of General Movements , 2008, Pediatric Research.

[14]  Yehezkel Ben-Ari,et al.  Rapid cortical oscillations and early motor activity in premature human neonate. , 2007, Cerebral cortex.

[15]  Marco Del Giudice,et al.  Alone in the dark? Modeling the conditions for visual experience in human fetuses. , 2011, Developmental psychobiology.

[16]  B. S. Kisilevsky,et al.  Human Fetal Behavior: 100 Years of Study☆☆☆ , 1998 .

[17]  Brian Francis,et al.  Do fetuses move their lips to the sound that they hear? An observational feasibility study on auditory stimulation in the womb , 2016, Pilot and Feasibility Studies.

[18]  A. V. D. van der Meer Keeping the arm in the limelight: advanced visual control of arm movements in neonates. , 1997, European journal of paediatric neurology : EJPN : official journal of the European Paediatric Neurology Society.

[19]  M. Feller,et al.  Spontaneous Correlated Activity in Developing Neural Circuits , 1999, Neuron.

[20]  Gilbert Gottlieb,et al.  Experiential canalization of behavioral development: results , 1991 .

[21]  R. Natale,et al.  Longitudinal measurements of fetal breathing, body movements, heart rate, and heart rate accelerations and decelerations at 24 to 32 weeks of gestation. , 1985, American journal of obstetrics and gynecology.

[22]  G. Müller,et al.  Embryonic motility: environmental influences and evolutionary innovation , 2003, Evolution & development.

[23]  C. Palmer,et al.  The discriminating nature of infants' exploratory actions , 1989 .

[24]  Anne Fernald,et al.  Peekaboo across Cultures: How Mothers and Infants Play with Voices, Faces, and Expectations , 1993 .

[25]  U. Hanaoka,et al.  Three‐dimensional sonographic assessment of fetal behavior in the early second trimester of pregnancy , 2001, Journal of ultrasound in medicine : official journal of the American Institute of Ultrasound in Medicine.

[26]  Mijna Hadders-Algra,et al.  Ontogeny of the human central nervous system: what is happening when? , 2006, Early human development.

[27]  G. Visser,et al.  The emergence of fetal behaviour. II. Quantitative aspects. , 1985, Early human development.

[28]  E. Thelen,et al.  The transition to reaching: mapping intention and intrinsic dynamics. , 1993, Child development.

[29]  G. Malinger,et al.  The corpus callosum: normal fetal development as shown by transvaginal sonography. , 1993, AJR. American journal of roentgenology.

[30]  Claes von Hofsten,et al.  The Organization of Arm and Hand Movements in the Neonate , 1989 .

[31]  A. Decasper,et al.  Lateralized processes constrain auditory reinforcement in human newborns , 2009, Hearing Research.

[32]  Asim Kurjak,et al.  The assessment of fetal neurobehavior by three-dimensional and four-dimensional ultrasound , 2008, The journal of maternal-fetal & neonatal medicine : the official journal of the European Association of Perinatal Medicine, the Federation of Asia and Oceania Perinatal Societies, the International Society of Perinatal Obstetricians.

[33]  Vasudevi Reddy,et al.  Infant clowns: The interpersonal creation of humour in infancy , 2001 .

[34]  C. K. Rovee,et al.  Conjugate reinforcement of infant exploratory behavior. , 1969, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[35]  Ian Q. Whishaw,et al.  Independent digit movements and precision grip patterns in 1–5-month-old human infants: hand-babbling, including vacuous then self-directed hand and digit movements, precedes targeted reaching , 2003, Neuropsychologia.

[36]  Vasudevi Reddy,et al.  Humor in Infants: Developmental and Psychological Perspectives , 2016 .

[37]  Séverine Henry,et al.  Unexpected behavioural consequences of preterm newborns' clothing , 2015, Scientific Reports.

[38]  A. Jacquet,et al.  Decelerative Cardiac Responsiveness to Acoustical Stimulation in the near Term Fetus , 1992, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. B, Comparative and physiological psychology.

[39]  C. Hofsten,et al.  Preparation for grasping an object: a developmental study. , 1988, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[40]  Hubert Preissl,et al.  Auditory habituation in the fetus and neonate: an fMEG study. , 2013, Developmental science.

[41]  Yasuo Kuniyoshi,et al.  A Fetus and Infant Developmental Scenario: Self-organization of Goal-directed Behaviors Based on Sensory Constraints , 2010, EpiRob.

[42]  Jacqueline Fagard,et al.  Changes in reaching and grasping objects of different sizes between 7 and 13 months of age , 1996 .

[43]  L. Fetters,et al.  Quantitative assessment of infant reaching movements. , 1987, Journal of motor behavior.

[44]  H. Prechtl,et al.  Classification and quantitative aspects of fetal movements during the second half of normal pregnancy. , 1991, Early human development.

[45]  Jenni M. Karl,et al.  Independent development of the Reach and the Grasp in spontaneous self-touching by human infants in the first 6 months , 2015, Front. Psychol..

[46]  Guillermo Azumendi,et al.  Behavioral pattern continuity from prenatal to postnatal life a study by four-dimensional (4D) ultrasonography , 2004, Journal of perinatal medicine.

[47]  Hideko Takeshita,et al.  Do Human Fetuses Anticipate Self‐Oriented Actions? A Study by Four‐Dimensional (4D) Ultrasonography , 2006 .

[48]  Anton R Dewsbury What the fetus feels , 1980 .

[49]  A. Kurjak,et al.  Prenatal diagnosis: what does four-dimensional ultrasound add? , 2002, Journal of perinatal medicine.

[50]  Paolo Dario,et al.  Longitudinal study of unimanual actions and grasping forces during infancy. , 2012, Infant behavior & development.

[51]  C. Hofsten Eye–hand coordination in the newborn. , 1982 .

[52]  Mijna Hadders-Algra,et al.  Putative neural substrate of normal and abnormal general movements , 2007, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[53]  J. Konczak,et al.  The development of goal-directed reaching in infants: hand trajectory formation and joint torque control , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[54]  J. O'Regan,et al.  Which limb is it? Responses to vibrotactile stimulation in early infancy , 2017, The British journal of developmental psychology.

[55]  Gudrun Schwarzer,et al.  Ten- and twelve-month-olds' visual anticipation of orientation and size during grasping. , 2011, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[56]  C. Palmer,et al.  The Discriminating Nature of Infants' Exploratory Actions. , 1989 .

[57]  Domenico Parisi,et al.  Using Motor Babbling and Hebb Rules for Modeling the Development of Reaching with Obstacles and Grasping , 2008 .

[58]  H. Ruff Infants' Manipulative Exploration of Objects: Effects of Age and Object Characteristics. , 1984 .

[59]  G. Michel,et al.  Development of role-differentiated bimanual manipulation during the infant's first year. , 2010, Developmental psychobiology.

[60]  J. W. Sparling,et al.  Fetal and neonatal hand movement. , 1999, Physical therapy.

[61]  Katharina J. Rohlfing,et al.  Taking Up an Active Role: Emerging Participation in Early Mother–Infant Interaction during Peekaboo Routines , 2017, Front. Psychol..

[62]  R. Mccall,et al.  Exploratory manipulation and play in the human infant. , 1974, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.

[63]  M. Busnel,et al.  Differential fetal auditory reactiveness as a function of stimulus characteristics and state. , 1989, Seminars in perinatology.

[64]  Tim Donovan,et al.  The Human Fetus Preferentially Engages with Face-like Visual Stimuli , 2018, Current Biology.

[65]  P. Orgeur,et al.  Olfaction in Utero: Can the Rodent Model be Generalized? , 1992, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. B, Comparative and physiological psychology.

[66]  Gentaro Taga,et al.  General to specific development of movement patterns and memory for contingency between actions and events in young infants. , 2006, Infant behavior & development.

[67]  Marla B. Feller,et al.  A Role for Correlated Spontaneous Activity in the Assembly of Neural Circuits , 2013, Neuron.

[68]  Davenport Hooker,et al.  The prenatal origin of behavior , 1952 .

[69]  A. Piontelli,et al.  Development of Normal Fetal Movements , 2010 .

[70]  M. Gattis,et al.  Do the wrong thing: How toddlers tell a joke from a mistake , 2008 .

[71]  T. Bower,et al.  Demonstration of Intention in the Reaching Behaviour of Neonate Humans , 1970, Nature.

[72]  A. Piontelli Development of Normal Fetal Movements: The First 25 Weeks of Gestation , 2010 .