Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation of human area MT/V5 disrupts perception and storage of the motion aftereffect

Following adaptation to a moving stimulus, the introduction of a stationary pattern creates the illusion of motion. This phenomenon, known as the motion aftereffect (MAE), can be delayed by placing a blank storage interval between the adapting and test stimuli. Human motion selective area MT/V5 has been proposed as the likely neural origin of MAEs. To examine the role of MT/V5 in perceiving and storing MAEs, we applied repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) to this area during a 10s storage interval and while subjects perceived illusory motion. Our results show that rTMS disrupts perception of the MAE when it is delivered in the early parts of the storage period and when it is applied during the perceptual MAE itself. Stimulation of control regions corresponding to V1 or Cz did not affect the MAE. In addition, magnetic stimulation of dorsolateral prefrontal and posterior parietal cortices did not disrupt MAE perception. These data provide experimental support for the notion that MT/V5 subserves perception and storage of the motion aftereffect.

[1]  S. Petersen,et al.  Direction-specific adaptation in area MT of the owl monkey , 1985, Brain Research.

[2]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  Area V5 of the human brain: evidence from a combined study using positron emission tomography and magnetic resonance imaging. , 1993, Cerebral cortex.

[3]  P M Rossini,et al.  Interhemispheric asymmetries in the perception of unimanual and bimanual cutaneous stimuli. A study using transcranial magnetic stimulation. , 1999, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[4]  Á. Pascual-Leone,et al.  Fast Backprojections from the Motion to the Primary Visual Area Necessary for Visual Awareness , 2001, Science.

[5]  H. Müller-Gärtner,et al.  The motion aftereffect: more than area V5/MT? Evidence from 15O-butanol PET studies , 2001, Brain Research.

[6]  V. Hömberg,et al.  Cerebral visual motion blindness: transitory akinetopsia induced by transcranial magnetic stimulation of human area V5 , 1992, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[7]  G. Mather The Movement Aftereffect and a Distribution-Shift Model for Coding the Direction of Visual Movement , 1980, Perception.

[8]  Frank Padberg,et al.  Transcranial magnetic stimulation in therapy studies: examination of the reliability of “standard” coil positioning by neuronavigation , 2001, Biological Psychiatry.

[9]  S. Shimojo,et al.  Manifestation of scotomas created by transcranial magnetic stimulation of human visual cortex , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[10]  Mark Hallett,et al.  Two periods of processing in the (circum)striate visual cortex as revealed by transcranial magnetic stimulation , 1998, Neuropsychologia.

[11]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  A direct demonstration of functional specialization in human visual cortex , 1991, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[12]  P. Thompson,et al.  The Role of Intervening Patterns in the Storage of the Movement Aftereffect , 1994, Perception.

[13]  A. Chaudhuri Modulation of the motion aftereffect by selective attention , 1990, Nature.

[14]  I. M. Spigel The Effects of Differential Post-Exposure Illumination on the Decay of a Movement After-Effect , 1960 .

[15]  U. Ziemann,et al.  Transient visual field defects induced by transcranial magnetic stimulation over human occipital pole , 1998, Experimental Brain Research.

[16]  A. Cowey,et al.  Magnetically induced phosphenes in sighted, blind and blindsighted observers , 2000, Neuroreport.

[17]  R. Vautin,et al.  Responses of single cells in cat visual cortex to prolonged stimulus movement: neural correlates of visual aftereffects. , 1977, Journal of neurophysiology.

[18]  A. Cowey,et al.  Motion perception and perceptual learning studied by magnetic stimulation. , 1999, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology. Supplement.

[19]  G. Kerr,et al.  The effect of electromagnetic stimulation of the posterior parietal cortex on eye movements , 1992, Eye.

[20]  Á. Pascual-Leone,et al.  Enhanced visual spatial attention ipsilateral to rTMS-induced 'virtual lesions' of human parietal cortex , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[21]  Frans A. J. Verstraten,et al.  The motion aftereffect , 1998, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[22]  R. Andersen,et al.  Functional analysis of human MT and related visual cortical areas using magnetic resonance imaging , 1995, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[23]  S. Anand,et al.  The selectivity and timing of motion processing in human temporo–parieto–occipital and occipital cortex: a transcranial magnetic stimulation study , 1998, Neuropsychologia.

[24]  Alan Cowey,et al.  Transcranial magnetic stimulation and cognitive neuroscience , 2000, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[25]  S. Kosslyn,et al.  The role of area 17 in visual imagery: convergent evidence from PET and rTMS. , 1999, Science.

[26]  Sarah H Lisanby,et al.  Therapeutic application of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation: a review , 2001, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[27]  A P Rudell,et al.  Transcranial magnetic stimulation in study of the visual pathway. , 1998, Journal of clinical neurophysiology : official publication of the American Electroencephalographic Society.

[28]  A. Dale,et al.  Visual motion aftereffect in human cortical area MT revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging , 1995, Nature.

[29]  E. R. Cohen,et al.  Close correlation between activity in brain area MT/V5 and the perception of a visual motion aftereffect , 1998, Current Biology.

[30]  K. Ziemons,et al.  The Network of Brain Areas Involved in the Motion Aftereffect , 2000, NeuroImage.

[31]  Ravi S. Menon,et al.  Recovery of fMRI activation in motion area MT following storage of the motion aftereffect. , 1999, Journal of neurophysiology.

[32]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  A new anatomical landmark for reliable identification of human area V5/MT: a quantitative analysis of sulcal patterning. , 2000, Cerebral cortex.

[33]  A. Cowey,et al.  The role of the parietal cortex in visual attention—hemispheric asymmetries and the effects of learning: a magnetic stimulation study , 1998, Neuropsychologia.

[34]  Alexander Grunewald,et al.  Orthogonal motion after-effect illusion predicted by a model of cortical motion processing , 1996, Nature.

[35]  L. Cohen,et al.  Enhanced excitability of the human visual cortex induced by short-term light deprivation. , 2000, Cerebral cortex.

[36]  A. T. Smith,et al.  Motion after-effects in cat striate cortex elicited by moving gratings , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[37]  Matthew Flatt,et al.  PsyScope: An interactive graphic system for designing and controlling experiments in the psychology laboratory using Macintosh computers , 1993 .

[38]  D. Heeger,et al.  Neuronal Basis of the Motion Aftereffect Reconsidered , 2001, Neuron.

[39]  S. Zeki,et al.  The consequences of inactivating areas V1 and V5 on visual motion perception. , 1995, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[40]  D. Braun,et al.  Transcranial magnetic stimulation of extrastriate cortex degrades human motion direction discrimination , 1994, Vision Research.

[41]  S. Anstis,et al.  Separate motion aftereffects from each eye and from both eyes , 1983, Vision Research.