Response artifacts in the measurement of subjective well-being

The degree to which response artifacts introduce error into self-report measures has long been a matter of concern in the psychological literature. For example, it has been suggested that self-report measures of subjective well-being (SWB) contain large amounts of variance due to the response style of social desirability (Carstensen and Cone, 1983). In the present study, four potential response artifacts (social desirability, current mood, moral beliefs about happiness, and happiness image management) and their effects on self reports of SWB were studied. Using nonself-report measures of happiness, in addition to self-report measures of SWB, various modes for the prediction of SWB were constructed. A measure of social desirability was found to be a significant predictor of nonself-report as well as self-reported measures of happiness, indicating that social desirability is a substantive personality characteristic which enhances well-being, rather than being a response artifact and source of error variance. Current mood was found to sometimes contribute as a predictor of self-report measures of SWB, suggesting the need for control of or assessment of its effects. Moral beliefs in happiness and image management did not significantly correlate with measures of happiness. Implications of the results for the measurement of well-being and for future research are discussed.

[1]  Frank M. Andrews,et al.  Social Indicators of Well-Being , 1976 .

[2]  Frank M. Andrews,et al.  Social Indicators of Well-Being: Americans' Perceptions of Life Quality , 1976 .

[3]  T. Moum,et al.  Yea-saying and mood-of-the-day effects in self-reported quality of life , 1988 .

[4]  A. L. Edwards The measurement of personality traits by scales and inventories , 1970 .

[5]  M. Stones,et al.  Social desirability in measures of subjective well-being: Age comparisons , 1988 .

[6]  Seymour Sudman,et al.  Improving Interview Method and Questionnaire Design. , 1982 .

[7]  J. S. Wiggins,et al.  Personality and Prediction: Principles of Personality Assessment , 1973 .

[8]  L. Carstensen,et al.  Social desirability and the measurement of psychological well-being in elderly persons. , 1983, Journal of gerontology.

[9]  A. Furnham Response bias, social desirability and dissimulation , 1986 .

[10]  G. Clore,et al.  Mood, misattribution, and judgments of well-being: Informative and directive functions of affective states. , 1983 .

[11]  G. A. Miller,et al.  Book Review Nisbett, R. , & Ross, L.Human inference: Strategies and shortcomings of social judgment.Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall, 1980. , 1982 .

[12]  G. Bower Mood and memory. , 1981, The American psychologist.

[13]  M. Stones,et al.  Social desirability in measures of subjective well-being: a systematic evaluation. , 1987, Journal of gerontology.

[14]  L. Ross,et al.  Human Inference: Strategies and Shortcomings of Social Judgment. , 1981 .

[15]  R. Arkin,et al.  Plumbing the Depths of the Bogus Pipeline: A Reprise. , 1983 .

[16]  Timothy D. Wilson,et al.  Telling more than we can know: Verbal reports on mental processes. , 1977 .

[17]  E. Diener,et al.  Subjective well-being. , 1984, Psychological bulletin.

[18]  Paul T. Costa,et al.  Social desirability scales: More substance than style. , 1983 .

[19]  A. L. Edwards The social desirability variable in personality assessment and research , 1958 .

[20]  R. McCrae,et al.  Well-being scales do not measure social desirability. , 1986, Journal of gerontology.

[21]  R. Gottsdanker The Birren hypothesis of generalized slowing of the central nervous system with age. , 1985, Journal of gerontology.

[22]  D. Marlowe,et al.  The Approval Motive: Studies in Evaluative Dependence , 1980 .