Downward Gazing for Steadiness

When walking on an uneven surface or complex terrain, humans tend to gaze downward. Previous investigations indicate that visual information can be used for online control of stepping. Behavioral investigations suggest that, during walking, the availability of visual information increases stepping accuracy, but probably through a feedforward control mechanism. Consequently, downward gazing (DWG) is usually interpreted as a strategy used to acquire useful information for online and/or feedforward control of stepping. Visual information is not exclusively used for guiding locomotion; a wealth of literature has been published on the usefulness of visual information for feedback postural control. Critically, postural control has been shown to be sensitive to the visual flow arising from the respective motion of the individual and the 3D environment. To investigate whether DWG can be used to enhance feedback control of posture, rather than feedforward/online control of gait, we conducted a series of experiments that explore this possible interplay. Through these experiments we were able to show that DWG, just a few steps ahead, results in a steadier standing and walking posture, without the need for accuracy. Moreover, we were able to demonstrate that humans resort to DWG when walking stability is compromised, even when destabilizing features were visually unpredictable. This series of experiments provides sufficient evidence of the possible interplay between visual information used for guiding locomotion and that used for postural control. Moreover, this evidence raises concerns regarding the way we interpret gaze behavior without the knowledge of the type and use of the information gathered.

[1]  Y. Parmet,et al.  Treading on the unknown increases prefrontal activity: A pilot fNIRS study. , 2019, Gait & posture.

[2]  B. Fajen,et al.  Humans exploit the biomechanics of bipedal gait during visually guided walking over complex terrain , 2013, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[3]  D. Elliott,et al.  Does head extension and flexion increase postural instability in elderly subjects when visual information is kept constant? , 2005, Gait & posture.

[4]  W. Young,et al.  Evidence of a Link Between Fall-Related Anxiety and High-Risk Patterns of Visual Search in Older Adults During Adaptive Locomotion , 2019, The journals of gerontology. Series A, Biological sciences and medical sciences.

[5]  A. R. Den Otter,et al.  Why you need to look where you step for precise foot placement: the effects of gaze eccentricity on stepping errors. , 2013, Gait & posture.

[6]  M. Hollands,et al.  Gaze Behavior of Young and Older Adults During Stair Walking , 2009, Journal of motor behavior.

[7]  William H. Warren,et al.  Optic flow is used to control human walking , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[8]  Brett R Fajen,et al.  Visual control of foot placement when walking over complex terrain , 2014, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[9]  William H. Warren,et al.  Chapter 8 – Self-Motion: Visual Perception and Visual Control , 1995 .

[10]  A. Bronstein,et al.  Reorientation of visually evoked postural responses by different eye-in-orbit and head-on-trunk angular positions , 1996, Experimental Brain Research.

[11]  J. Carmichael Correspondence , 1884, Edinburgh Medical Journal.

[12]  B A Kay,et al.  Visual control of posture during walking: functional specificity. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[13]  Joan N. Vickers,et al.  How far ahead do we look when required to step on specific locations in the travel path during locomotion? , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.

[14]  Brett R Fajen,et al.  The biomechanics of walking shape the use of visual information during locomotion over complex terrain. , 2015, Journal of vision.

[15]  W. Young,et al.  The influence of anxiety and attentional focus on visual search during adaptive gait. , 2019, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[16]  Itshak Melzer,et al.  A retrospective analysis of balance control parameters in elderly fallers and non-fallers. , 2010, Clinical biomechanics.

[17]  Tom Chau,et al.  Managing variability in the summary and comparison of gait data , 2005, Journal of NeuroEngineering and Rehabilitation.

[18]  Holger Kantz,et al.  Practical implementation of nonlinear time series methods: The TISEAN package. , 1998, Chaos.

[19]  Robert J. Peterka,et al.  Postural control model interpretation of stabilogram diffusion analysis , 2000, Biological Cybernetics.

[20]  Jane E. Clark,et al.  On becoming skillful: patterns and constraints. , 1995, Research quarterly for exercise and sport.

[21]  Daniel S Marigold,et al.  Adaptive Gaze Strategies to Reduce Environmental Uncertainty During a Sequential Visuomotor Behaviour , 2018, Scientific Reports.

[22]  Paul R. Schrater,et al.  Perceiving visual expansion without optic flow , 2001, Nature.

[23]  M. Rosenstein,et al.  A practical method for calculating largest Lyapunov exponents from small data sets , 1993 .

[24]  T. Stoffregen Flow structure versus retinal location in the optical control of stance. , 1985, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[25]  Jonathan B Dingwell,et al.  How humans use visual optic flow to regulate stepping during walking. , 2017, Gait & posture.

[26]  M. Guerraz,et al.  Influence of motion parallax in the control of spontaneous body sway , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.

[27]  B. Day,et al.  Rapid visuo-motor processes drive the leg regardless of balance constraints , 2005, Current Biology.

[28]  Mary M. Hayhoe,et al.  Gaze and the Control of Foot Placement When Walking in Natural Terrain , 2018, Current Biology.

[29]  D. Marigold Role of Peripheral Visual Cues in Online Visual Guidance of Locomotion , 2008, Exercise and sport sciences reviews.

[30]  A. E. Patla,et al.  Gaze fixation patterns for negotiating complex ground terrain , 2007, Neuroscience.

[31]  Benoît G. Bardy,et al.  Motion parallax is used to control postural sway during walking , 1996, Experimental Brain Research.

[32]  H. Abarbanel,et al.  Determining embedding dimension for phase-space reconstruction using a geometrical construction. , 1992, Physical review. A, Atomic, molecular, and optical physics.

[33]  Mary M Hayhoe,et al.  Control of gaze in natural environments: effects of rewards and costs, uncertainty and memory in target selection , 2018, Interface Focus.

[34]  Y. Parmet,et al.  Treading on the unknown-the feasibility of a novel approach to investigating the motor control of walking. , 2018, Physiological measurement.

[35]  I. Melzer,et al.  Postural stability in the elderly: a comparison between fallers and non-fallers. , 2004, Age and ageing.

[36]  N. Harries,et al.  Training to walk amid uncertainty with Re-Step: measurements and changes with perturbation training for hemiparesis and cerebral palsy , 2013, Disability and rehabilitation. Assistive technology.

[37]  Raymond F Reynolds,et al.  Visual guidance of the human foot during a step , 2005, The Journal of physiology.

[38]  P. D. Drummond,et al.  A ATOMIC , MOLECULAR , AND OPTICAL PHYSICS , 1999 .

[39]  F. Takens Detecting strange attractors in turbulence , 1981 .

[40]  氏名 青木 Influence of gaze distance and downward gazing on postural sway in hemiplegic stroke patients , 2014 .

[41]  Scott A. England,et al.  The influence of gait speed on local dynamic stability of walking. , 2007, Gait & posture.

[42]  J. Dingwell,et al.  Nonlinear time series analysis of normal and pathological human walking. , 2000, Chaos.

[43]  T.E. Prieto,et al.  Measures of postural steadiness: differences between healthy young and elderly adults , 1996, IEEE Transactions on Biomedical Engineering.

[44]  The effects of various visual conditions on trunk control during ambulation in chronic post stroke patients. , 2017, Gait & posture.

[45]  David N. Lee Visual proprioceptive control of stance , 1975 .

[46]  A. Wolf,et al.  Determining Lyapunov exponents from a time series , 1985 .

[47]  M. Guerraz,et al.  Ocular versus extraocular control of posture and equilibrium , 2008, Neurophysiologie Clinique/Clinical Neurophysiology.

[48]  A E Patla,et al.  Where and when do we look as we approach and step over an obstacle in the travel path? , 1997, Neuroreport.

[49]  Aftab E. Patla,et al.  Review article Understanding the roles of vision in the control of human locomotion , 1997 .

[50]  Peter J. Beek,et al.  Statistical precision and sensitivity of measures of dynamic gait stability , 2009, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[51]  Zoï Kapoula,et al.  Effects of distance and gaze position on postural stability in young and old subjects , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.

[52]  J. J. Collins,et al.  Age-related changes in open-loop and closed-loop postural control mechanisms , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[53]  Bruce A. Kay,et al.  Coupling of posture and gait: mode locking and parametric excitation , 2001, Biological Cybernetics.

[54]  J. Collins,et al.  Open-loop and closed-loop control of posture: A random-walk analysis of center-of-pressure trajectories , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[55]  D. E. Marple-Horvat,et al.  Human Eye Movements During Visually Guided Stepping. , 1995, Journal of motor behavior.

[56]  Fraser,et al.  Independent coordinates for strange attractors from mutual information. , 1986, Physical review. A, General physics.

[57]  P. Beek,et al.  Assessing the stability of human locomotion: a review of current measures , 2013, Journal of The Royal Society Interface.