Canceling planned action: an FMRI study of countermanding saccades.

We investigated the voluntary control of motor behavior by studying the process of deciding whether or not to execute a movement. We imaged the human dorsal cortex while subjects performed a countermanding task that allowed us to manipulate the probability that subjects would be able to cancel a planned saccade in response to an imperative stop signal. We modeled the behavioral data as a race between gaze-shifting mechanisms and gaze-holding mechanisms towards a finish line where a saccade is generated or canceled, and estimated that saccade cancelation took approximately 160 ms. The frontal eye fields showed greater activation on stop signal trials regardless of successful cancelation, suggesting coactivation of saccade and fixation mechanisms. The supplementary eye fields, however, distinguished between successful and unsuccessful cancelation, suggesting a role in monitoring performance. These oculomotor regions play distinct roles in the decision processes mediating saccadic choice.

[1]  P. C. Murphy,et al.  Cerebral Cortex , 2017, Cerebral Cortex.

[2]  Jeffrey D Schall,et al.  On the role of frontal eye field in guiding attention and saccades , 2004, Vision Research.

[3]  Clayton E. Curtis,et al.  Maintenance of Spatial and Motor Codes during Oculomotor Delayed Response Tasks , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[4]  T. Robbins,et al.  Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[5]  P. Glimcher The neurobiology of visual-saccadic decision making. , 2003, Annual review of neuroscience.

[6]  Katherine M. Armstrong,et al.  Visuomotor Origins of Covert Spatial Attention , 2003, Neuron.

[7]  B A J Reddi,et al.  Accuracy, information, and response time in a saccadic decision task. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[8]  Joshua W. Brown,et al.  Performance Monitoring by the Anterior Cingulate Cortex During Saccade Countermanding , 2003, Science.

[9]  C. Curtis,et al.  Persistent activity in the prefrontal cortex during working memory , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[10]  D. P. Hanes,et al.  Controlled Movement Processing: Superior Colliculus Activity Associated with Countermanded Saccades , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[11]  Jillian H. Fecteau,et al.  Exploring the consequences of the previous trial , 2003, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[12]  C. Curtis,et al.  Success and Failure Suppressing Reflexive Behavior , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[13]  Hans Colonius,et al.  Countermanding saccades: Evidence against independent processing of go and stop signals , 2003, Perception & psychophysics.

[14]  Thomas E. Nichols,et al.  Optimization of experimental design in fMRI: a general framework using a genetic algorithm , 2003, NeuroImage.

[15]  T. Robbins,et al.  Stop-signal inhibition disrupted by damage to right inferior frontal gyrus in humans , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.

[16]  Ravi S. Menon,et al.  Human fMRI evidence for the neural correlates of preparatory set , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.

[17]  Andrew D. Jones,et al.  A computational model of anterior cingulate function in speeded response tasks: Effects of frequency, sequence, and conflict , 2002, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[18]  Joshua W. Brown,et al.  Monitoring and Control of Action by the Frontal Lobes , 2002, Neuron.

[19]  Clay B. Holroyd,et al.  The neural basis of human error processing: reinforcement learning, dopamine, and the error-related negativity. , 2002, Psychological review.

[20]  I. Johnsrude,et al.  The problem of functional localization in the human brain , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[21]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[22]  P. Strick,et al.  Imaging the premotor areas , 2001, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[23]  T. Mergner,et al.  Relationship between saccadic eye movements and cortical activity as measured by fMRI: quantitative and qualitative aspects , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.

[24]  R. Carpenter,et al.  Saccadic countermanding: a comparison of central and peripheral stop signals , 2001, Vision Research.

[25]  Hans Colonius,et al.  Countermanding saccades with auditory stop signals: testing the race model , 2001, Vision Research.

[26]  M. Botvinick,et al.  Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. , 2001, Psychological review.

[27]  T. Paus Primate anterior cingulate cortex: Where motor control, drive and cognition interface , 2001, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[28]  Clay B. Holroyd,et al.  Why is there an ERN/Ne on correct trials? Response representations, stimulus-related components, and the theory of error-processing , 2001, Biological Psychology.

[29]  J. Schall,et al.  Performance monitoring by the supplementary eye field , 2000, Nature.

[30]  D. Munoz,et al.  Control of saccade initiation in a countermanding task using visual and auditory stop signals , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.

[31]  M. Posner,et al.  Cognitive and emotional influences in anterior cingulate cortex , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[32]  R. Carpenter,et al.  The neural control of looking , 2000, Current Biology.

[33]  D. Gitelman,et al.  Covert Visual Spatial Orienting and Saccades: Overlapping Neural Systems , 2000, NeuroImage.

[34]  D. Gitelman,et al.  Neuroanatomic Overlap of Working Memory and Spatial Attention Networks: A Functional MRI Comparison within Subjects , 1999, NeuroImage.

[35]  A. Berthoz,et al.  An anatomical landmark for the supplementary eye fields in human revealed with functional magnetic resonance imaging. , 1999, Cerebral cortex.

[36]  R. Carpenter,et al.  Countermanding saccades in humans , 1999, Vision Research.

[37]  Joel R. Meyer,et al.  A large-scale distributed network for covert spatial attention: further anatomical delineation based on stringent behavioural and cognitive controls. , 1999, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[38]  J. Schall,et al.  Role of frontal eye fields in countermanding saccades: visual, movement, and fixation activity. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[39]  J. Schall,et al.  Countermanding saccades in macaque , 1995, Visual Neuroscience.

[40]  G D Logan,et al.  Strategies and mechanisms in nonselective and selective inhibitory motor control. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[41]  P. Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Prefrontal connections of medial motor areas in the rhesus monkey , 1993, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[42]  E. Donchin,et al.  Optimizing the use of information: strategic control of activation of responses. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[43]  M G Coles,et al.  On the transmission of partial information: inferences from movement-related brain potentials. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[44]  J. Kaas,et al.  Supplementary eye field as defined by intracortical microstimulation: Connections in macaques , 1990, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[45]  G. Logan,et al.  In search of the point of no return: the control of response processes. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[46]  D. Meyer,et al.  The point of no return in choice reaction time: controlled and ballistic stages of response preparation. , 1986, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[47]  G. Logan On the ability to inhibit thought and action , 1984 .

[48]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Spatial attention and eye movements , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[49]  R. Andersen,et al.  Intentional maps in posterior parietal cortex. , 2002, Annual review of neuroscience.

[50]  J. Schall Neural basis of deciding, choosing and acting , 2001, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[51]  Stephen Monsell,et al.  Control of Cognitive Processes , 2000 .

[52]  Stephen Monsell,et al.  Banishing the Control Homunculus , 2000 .

[53]  J. Schall,et al.  Neural control of behavior: countermanding eye movements , 2000, Psychological research.

[54]  M. Goldberg,et al.  Space and attention in parietal cortex. , 1999, Annual review of neuroscience.

[55]  B. J. McCurtain,et al.  Dorsal cortical regions subserving visually guided saccades in humans: an fMRI study. , 1998, Cerebral cortex.

[56]  M. Schlag-Rey,et al.  Evidence for a supplementary eye field. , 1987, Journal of neurophysiology.

[57]  American Psychological Association, Inc. On the Ability to Inhibit Simple and Choice Reaction Time Responses: A Model and a Method , 2022 .