Children's understanding of addition and subtraction concepts.

After the onset of formal schooling, little is known about the development of children's understanding of the arithmetic concepts of inversion and associativity. On problems of the form a+b-b (e.g., 3+26-26), if children understand the inversion concept (i.e., that addition and subtraction are inverse operations), then no calculations are needed to solve the problem. On problems of the form a+b-c (e.g., 3+27-23), if children understand the associativity concept (i.e., that the addition and subtraction can be solved in any order), then the second part of the problem can be solved first. Children in Grades 2, 3, and 4 solved both types of problems and then were given a demonstration of how to apply both concepts. Approval of each concept and preference of a conceptual approach versus an algorithmic approach were measured. Few grade differences were found on either task. Conceptual understanding was greater for inversion than for associativity on both tasks. Clusters of participants in all grades showed that some had strong understanding of both concepts, some had strong understanding of the inversion concept only, and others had weak understanding of both concepts. The findings highlight the lack of developmental increases and the large individual differences in conceptual understanding on two arithmetic concepts during the early school years.

[1]  Katherine M Robinson,et al.  Adults' understanding of inversion concepts: how does performance on addition and subtraction inversion problems compare to performance on multiplication and division inversion problems? , 2003, Canadian journal of experimental psychology = Revue canadienne de psychologie experimentale.

[2]  David F. Bjorklund,et al.  Children's strategies: Contemporary views of cognitive development. , 1991 .

[3]  A. Baroody,et al.  Preschoolers' Understanding of the Addition–Subtraction Inverse Principle: A Taiwanese Sample , 2007 .

[4]  K. H. Canobi,et al.  Number words in young children’s conceptual and procedural knowledge of addition, subtraction and inversion , 2008, Cognition.

[5]  K. H. Canobi,et al.  Individual Differences in Children's Addition and Subtraction Knowledge. , 2004 .

[6]  J. Bisanz,et al.  Preschoolers doing arithmetic: the concepts are willing but the working memory is weak. , 2000, Canadian journal of experimental psychology = Revue canadienne de psychologie experimentale.

[7]  Ann Dowker,et al.  Individual differences in arithmetic , 2012, Individual Differences in Arithmetic.

[8]  Nicole M. McNeil U-shaped development in math: 7-year-olds outperform 9-year-olds on equivalence problems. , 2007, Developmental psychology.

[9]  R. Siegler Hazards of mental chronometry: An example from children's subtraction. , 1989 .

[10]  Peter Bryant,et al.  Individual differences in children's understanding of inversion and arithmetical skill. , 2006, The British journal of educational psychology.

[11]  Richard Cowan,et al.  Do They Know What They Are Doing? Children's Use of Economical Addition Strategies and Knowledge of Commutativity , 1996 .

[12]  Jeffrey Bisanz,et al.  Evidence for Use of Mathematical Inversion By Three-Year-Old Children , 2007 .

[13]  K. M. Robinson,et al.  Children's understanding of the arithmetic concepts of inversion and associativity. , 2006, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[14]  Arthur J. Baroody,et al.  Children's Use of Mathematical Structure. , 1983 .

[15]  Ann Dowker,et al.  The development of arithmetic concepts and skills: Constructing adaptive expertise , 2003 .

[16]  Jeffrey Bisanz,et al.  Use of the mathematical principle of inversion in young children. , 2003, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[17]  K. H. Canobi,et al.  Children's profiles of addition and subtraction understanding. , 2005, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[18]  Elizabeth S. Spelke,et al.  Children’s understanding of the relationship between addition and subtraction , 2008, Cognition.

[19]  Arthur J. Baroody The development of adaptive expertise and flexibility: The integration of conceptual and procedural knowledge , 2003 .

[20]  Arthur J. Baroody,et al.  An evaluation of evidence supporting fact-retrieval models , 1994 .

[21]  Elsbeth Stern,et al.  Spontaneous use of conceptual mathematical knowledge in elementary school children , 1992 .

[22]  Ann Dowker,et al.  The Development of Arithmetic Concepts and Skills , 2003 .

[23]  Jeffrey Bisanz,et al.  Strategic and nonstrategic processing in the development of mathematical cognition. , 1990 .

[24]  Jeffrey Bisanz,et al.  Chapter 3 Understanding Elementary Mathematics , 1992 .

[25]  Roberto Araya,et al.  A computational model of conscious and unconscious strategy discovery. , 2005, Advances in child development and behavior.

[26]  R. Siegler The perils of averaging data over strategies: An example from children's addition. , 1987 .

[27]  Robert A. Reeve,et al.  Young Children's Understanding of Addition Concepts , 2002 .

[28]  A. Su,et al.  The National Council of Teachers of Mathematics , 1932, The Mathematical Gazette.

[29]  Camilla K. Gilmore Investigating children's understanding of inversion using the missing number paradigm , 2006 .

[30]  P. Bryant,et al.  Children's understanding of the relation between addition and subtraction: inversion, identity, and decomposition. , 1999, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[31]  R. Siegler,et al.  Conscious and unconscious strategy discoveries: a microgenetic analysis. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[32]  P. Pattison,et al.  The role of conceptual understanding in children's addition problem solving. , 1998, Developmental psychology.

[33]  Katherine M. Robinson,et al.  The validity of verbal reports in children's subtraction , 2001 .