Differential responses in dorsal visual cortex to motion and disparity depth cues

We investigated how interactions between monocular motion parallax and binocular cues to depth vary in human motion areas for wide-field visual motion stimuli (110 × 100°). We used fMRI with an extensive 2 × 3 × 2 factorial blocked design in which we combined two types of self-motion (translational motion and translational + rotational motion), with three categories of motion inflicted by the degree of noise (self-motion, distorted self-motion, and multiple object-motion), and two different view modes of the flow patterns (stereo and synoptic viewing). Interactions between disparity and motion category revealed distinct contributions to self- and object-motion processing in 3D. For cortical areas V6 and CSv, but not the anterior part of MT+ with bilateral visual responsiveness (MT+/b), we found a disparity-dependent effect of rotational flow and noise: When self-motion perception was degraded by adding rotational flow and moderate levels of noise, the BOLD responses were reduced compared with translational self-motion alone, but this reduction was cancelled by adding stereo information which also rescued the subject's self-motion percept. At high noise levels, when the self-motion percept gave way to a swarm of moving objects, the BOLD signal strongly increased compared to self-motion in areas MT+/b and V6, but only for stereo in the latter. BOLD response did not increase for either view mode in CSv. These different response patterns indicate different contributions of areas V6, MT+/b, and CSv to the processing of self-motion perception and the processing of multiple independent motions.

[1]  D. V. Essen,et al.  Neural mechanisms of form and motion processing in the primate visual system , 1994, Neuron.

[2]  D. Heeger,et al.  Retinotopy and Functional Subdivision of Human Areas MT and MST , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[3]  Eli Brenner,et al.  Humans combine the optic flow with static depth cues for robust perception of heading , 1994, Vision Research.

[4]  G. Orban,et al.  The Retinotopic Organization of the Human Middle Temporal Area MT/V5 and Its Cortical Neighbors , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[5]  A. V. van den Berg,et al.  Adjacent visual representations of self-motion in different reference frames , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[6]  Hiroshi Ban,et al.  The integration of motion and disparity cues to depth in dorsal visual cortex , 2012, Nature Neuroscience.

[7]  Kenneth J. Ciuffreda,et al.  Why Two Eyes ? , 2005 .

[8]  Finnegan J. Calabro,et al.  Acoustic facilitation of object movement detection during self-motion , 2011, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[9]  A. V. van den Berg,et al.  Why two eyes are better than one for judgements of heading , 1994, Nature.

[10]  S Saida,et al.  How is motion disparity integrated with binocular disparity in depth perception? , 1996, Perception & psychophysics.

[11]  A. T. Smith,et al.  Sensitivity to optic flow in human cortical areas MT and MST , 2006, The European journal of neuroscience.

[12]  Velia Cardin,et al.  Human V6: Functional Characterisation and Localisation , 2012, PloS one.

[13]  M. Landy,et al.  Measurement and modeling of depth cue combination: in defense of weak fusion , 1995, Vision Research.

[14]  O. Braddick,et al.  Brain Areas Sensitive to Coherent Visual Motion , 2001, Perception.

[15]  J. Rovamo,et al.  An estimation and application of the human cortical magnification factor , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[16]  D. Heeger,et al.  Two Retinotopic Visual Areas in Human Lateral Occipital Cortex , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[17]  Andrew T. Smith,et al.  The Representation of Egomotion in the Human Brain , 2008, Current Biology.

[18]  S. Nishida,et al.  The dynamics of the visual system in combining conflicting KDE and binocular stereopsis cues , 1994, Perception & psychophysics.

[19]  Sabrina Pitzalis,et al.  Parallel motion signals to the medial and lateral motion areas V6 and MT+ , 2013, NeuroImage.

[20]  Ravi S. Menon,et al.  Distinguishing subregions of the human MT+ complex using visual fields and pursuit eye movements. , 2001, Journal of neurophysiology.

[21]  Georgios A. Keliris,et al.  A binocular rivalry study of motion perception in the human brain , 2005, Vision Research.

[22]  F. Bremmer,et al.  Perception of self-motion from visual flow , 1999, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[23]  Colin Blakemore,et al.  Areas of the human brain activated by ambient visual motion, indicating three kinds of self-movement , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.

[24]  S. Rushton,et al.  Optic Flow Processing for the Assessment of Object Movement during Ego Movement , 2009, Current Biology.

[25]  A. Berthoz,et al.  Functional MRI of galvanic vestibular stimulation. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[26]  Andreas Bartels,et al.  Visual Motion Responses in the Posterior Cingulate Sulcus: A Comparison to V5/MT and MST , 2011, Cerebral cortex.

[27]  Kenneth H. Britten,et al.  Mechanisms of self-motion perception. , 2008, Annual review of neuroscience.

[28]  B Rogers,et al.  Motion Parallax as an Independent Cue for Depth Perception , 1979, Perception.

[29]  Andrew T. Smith,et al.  Sensitivity of human visual cortical areas to the stereoscopic depth of a moving stimulus. , 2008, Journal of vision.

[30]  J W Belliveau,et al.  Borders of multiple visual areas in humans revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging. , 1995, Science.

[31]  Velia Cardin,et al.  Sensitivity of human visual cortical area V6 to stereoscopic depth gradients associated with self-motion , 2011, Journal of neurophysiology.

[32]  K. Grill-Spector,et al.  The human visual cortex. , 2004, Annual review of neuroscience.

[33]  S. Rushton,et al.  The pop out of scene-relative object movement against retinal motion due to self-movement , 2007, Cognition.

[34]  M. Fahle Wozu zwei Augen? , 1987, Naturwissenschaften.

[35]  B. Rogers,et al.  The Interaction of Binocular Disparity and Motion Parallax in the Computation of Depth , 1996, Vision Research.

[36]  S. Rushton,et al.  Moving observers, relative retinal motion and the detection of object movement , 2005, Current Biology.

[37]  Gregory C. DeAngelis,et al.  Vestibular Facilitation of Optic Flow Parsing , 2010, PloS one.

[38]  J. Koenderink,et al.  Facts on optic flow , 1987, Biological Cybernetics.

[39]  C. Galletti,et al.  Neuronal mechanisms for detection of motion in the field of view , 2003, Neuropsychologia.

[40]  F Bremmer,et al.  Eye position encoding in the macaque ventral intraparietal area (VIP). , 1999, Neuroreport.

[41]  François Klam,et al.  ã Federation of European Neuroscience Societies Visual±vestibular interactive responses in the macaque ventral intraparietal area (VIP) , 2022 .

[42]  D Regan,et al.  The relation between discrimination and sensitivity in the perception of motion in depth. , 1975, The Journal of physiology.

[43]  Colin W G Clifford,et al.  Decoding conjunctions of direction-of-motion and binocular disparity from human visual cortex. , 2012, Journal of neurophysiology.

[44]  B J Rogers,et al.  The Appearance of Surfaces Specified by Motion Parallax and Binocular Disparity , 1989, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[45]  Jonathan W. Kelly,et al.  Perception of heading without retinal optic flow , 2006, Perception & psychophysics.

[46]  Velia Cardin,et al.  Sensitivity of human visual and vestibular cortical regions to egomotion-compatible visual stimulation. , 2010, Cerebral cortex.

[47]  B. Wandell,et al.  Visual Field Maps in Human Cortex , 2007, Neuron.

[48]  C. Galletti,et al.  Wide-Field Retinotopy Defines Human Cortical Visual Area V6 , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[49]  Andreas Bartels,et al.  Human Areas V3A and V6 Compensate for Self-Induced Planar Visual Motion , 2012, Neuron.

[50]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  Identification of the central vestibular projections in man: a positron emission tomography activation study , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[51]  Thomas Haarmeier,et al.  Processing of Coherent Visual Motion in Topographically Organized Visual Areas in Human Cerebral Cortex , 2012, Brain Topography.

[52]  C. Galletti,et al.  Human V6: The Medial Motion Area , 2009, Cerebral cortex.

[53]  B. Rogers,et al.  Similarities between motion parallax and stereopsis in human depth perception , 1982, Vision Research.

[54]  Daniel J. Hannon,et al.  Direction of self-motion is perceived from optical flow , 1988, Nature.

[55]  D. Regan,et al.  Estimating the time to collision with a rotating nonspherical object , 2000, Vision Research.

[56]  Ning Qian,et al.  Binocular Disparity Review and the Perception of Depth , 1997 .

[57]  Gregory C. DeAngelis,et al.  A neural representation of depth from motion parallax in macaque visual cortex , 2008, Nature.

[58]  Fulvio Domini,et al.  Stereo and motion information are not independently processed by the visual system , 2006, Vision Research.

[59]  L. Cormack,et al.  Disparity- and velocity-based signals for three-dimensional motion perception in human MT+ , 2009, Nature Neuroscience.

[60]  D. Regan,et al.  Binocular correlates of the direction of motion in depth , 1993, Vision Research.

[61]  S. Rushton,et al.  Perception of scene-relative object movement: Optic flow parsing and the contribution of monocular depth cues , 2009, Vision Research.