Familiarity expands space and contracts time

When humans draw maps, or make judgments about travel‐time, their responses are rarely accurate and are often systematically distorted. Distortion effects on estimating time to arrival and the scale of sketch‐maps reveal the nature of mental representation of time and space. Inspired by data from rodent entorhinal grid cells, we predicted that familiarity to an environment would distort representations of the space by expanding the size of it. We also hypothesized that travel‐time estimation would be distorted in the same direction as space‐size, if time and space rely on the same cognitive map. We asked international students, who had lived at a college in London for 9 months, to sketch a south‐up map of their college district, estimate travel‐time to destinations within the area, and mark their everyday walking routes. We found that while estimates for sketched space were expanded with familiarity, estimates of the time to travel through the space were contracted with familiarity. Thus, we found dissociable responses to familiarity in representations of time and space. © 2016 The Authors Hippocampus Published by Wiley Periodicals, Inc.

[1]  Kazunori Hanyu,et al.  Cognitive Distance of Stairways , 1995 .

[2]  G. Winocur,et al.  The hippocampus is involved in mental navigation for a recently learned, but not a highly familiar environment: A longitudinal fMRI study , 2012, Hippocampus.

[3]  H. Eichenbaum,et al.  Hippocampal “Time Cells” Bridge the Gap in Memory for Discontiguous Events , 2011, Neuron.

[4]  Morris Moscovitch,et al.  Memory and navigation: Compression of space varies with route length and turns , 2016, Hippocampus.

[5]  T. Seno,et al.  Self-Motion Perception Compresses Time Experienced in Return Travel , 2011, Perception.

[6]  Hilla Peretz,et al.  Ju n 20 03 Schrödinger ’ s Cat : The rules of engagement , 2003 .

[7]  B. Kuipers The "Map in the Head" Metaphor , 1982 .

[8]  B Tversky,et al.  Descriptions and depictions of environments , 1992, Memory & cognition.

[9]  Michel Denis,et al.  Is Memory for Routes Enhanced by an Environment's Richness in Visual Landmarks? , 2014, Spatial Cogn. Comput..

[10]  W. Penny,et al.  Replay of Very Early Encoding Representations during Recollection , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[11]  N. Burgess,et al.  Evidence for holistic episodic recollection via hippocampal pattern completion , 2015, Nature Communications.

[12]  Reginald G. Golledge,et al.  Spatial Knowledge Acquisition by Children: Route Learning and Relational Distances , 1992 .

[13]  Mark P. Brandon,et al.  During Running in Place, Grid Cells Integrate Elapsed Time and Distance Run , 2015, Neuron.

[14]  I. Fried,et al.  Direct recordings of grid-like neuronal activity in human spatial navigation , 2013, Nature Neuroscience.

[15]  Erik A. Wing,et al.  Neural similarity between encoding and retrieval is related to memory via hippocampal interactions. , 2013, Cerebral cortex.

[16]  S. Kosslyn,et al.  Cognitive maps in children and men. , 1974, Child development.

[17]  Katherine R. Sherrill,et al.  Hippocampus and Retrosplenial Cortex Combine Path Integration Signals for Successful Navigation , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[18]  Tommy Gärling,et al.  Intraurban Cognitive Distance: The Relationship between Judgments of Straight-Line Distances, Travel Distances, and Travel Times , 2010 .

[19]  Arne D. Ekstrom,et al.  Spatial and temporal episodic memory retrieval recruit dissociable functional networks in the human brain. , 2007, Learning & memory.

[20]  H. Eichenbaum Time cells in the hippocampus: a new dimension for mapping memories , 2014, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[21]  J. O’Keefe,et al.  Grid cell firing patterns signal environmental novelty by expansion , 2012, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[22]  Neil Burgess,et al.  Using Grid Cells for Navigation , 2015, Neuron.

[23]  Neil Burgess,et al.  Grid-like Processing of Imagined Navigation , 2016, Current Biology.

[24]  D. R. Montello,et al.  Spatial knowledge acquisition from direct experience in the environment: Individual differences in the development of metric knowledge and the integration of separately learned places , 2006, Cognitive Psychology.

[25]  T. Rogers,et al.  Where do you know what you know? The representation of semantic knowledge in the human brain , 2007, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[26]  Jodie M. Plumert,et al.  Distance perception in real and virtual environments , 2004, APGV '04.

[27]  Daniel A. Gajewski,et al.  Medial Temporal Lobe Roles in Human Path Integration , 2014, PloS one.

[28]  M. Moscovitch,et al.  The effects of spatial contextual familiarity on remembered scenes, episodic memories, and imagined future events. , 2014, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[29]  R. Knight,et al.  The Hippocampus and Entorhinal Cortex Encode the Path and Euclidean Distances to Goals during Navigation , 2014, Current Biology.

[30]  Aiden E. G. F. Arnold,et al.  Mental simulation of routes during navigation involves adaptive temporal compression , 2016, Cognition.

[31]  The return trip effect: Why the return trip often seems to take less time , 2011, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[32]  Emilio Kropff,et al.  Place cells, grid cells, and the brain's spatial representation system. , 2008, Annual review of neuroscience.

[33]  M. Moscovitch,et al.  Impoverished descriptions of familiar routes in three cases of hippocampal/medial temporal lobe amnesia , 2015, Cortex.

[34]  J. Wohlwill Image and Environment. Cognitive Mapping and Spatial Behavior. Roger M. Downs and David Stea, Eds. Aldine, Chicago, 1973. xxiii, 440 pp., illus. $15 , 1974 .

[35]  Caswell Barry,et al.  Neural systems supporting navigation , 2015, Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences.