Spatiotemporal segregation in visual search: evidence from parietal lesions.
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] H. Pashler,et al. Visual selection mediated by location: Selecting successive visual objects , 1995, Perception & psychophysics.
[2] Lars Nyberg,et al. Brain Regions Differentially Involved in Remembering What and When: a PET Study , 1997, Neuron.
[3] R Blake,et al. Visual form created solely from temporal structure. , 1999, Science.
[4] Glyn W. Humphreys,et al. Interactions between object and space systems revealed through neuropsychology , 1993 .
[5] S. Palmer. Common region: A new principle of perceptual grouping , 1992, Cognitive Psychology.
[6] S. Yantis,et al. Abrupt visual onsets and selective attention: Evidence from visual search. , 1984 .
[7] M. Critchley. The parietal lobes , 1966 .
[8] G. Humphreys,et al. Visual marking: prioritizing selection for new objects by top-down attentional inhibition of old objects. , 1997, Psychological review.
[9] M. Posner,et al. How do the parietal lobes direct covert attention? , 1987, Neuropsychologia.
[10] Derrick G. Watson,et al. Aging and visual marking: selective deficits for moving stimuli. , 2002, Psychology and aging.
[11] Jan Theeuwes,et al. Visual marking of old objects , 1998 .
[12] J Theeuwes,et al. Effects of location and form cuing on the allocation of attention in the visual field. , 1989, Acta psychologica.
[13] Glyn W. Humphreys,et al. Evidence from unilateral visual neglect , 1995 .
[14] Derrick G. Watson,et al. Visual marking of moving objects: a role for top-down feature-based inhibition in selection. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[15] J. Driver,et al. Can Visual Neglect Operate in Object-centred Co-ordinates? An Affirmative Single-case Study , 1991 .
[16] E. Maguire. The cerebral representation of space: insights from functional imaging data , 1997, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[17] Glyn W. Humphreys,et al. An Onset Advantage without a Preview Benefit: Neuropsychological Evidence Separating Onset and Preview Effects in Search , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[18] C. Eriksen,et al. Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task , 1974 .
[19] J. Hoffman,et al. Spatial attention in vision , 1986, Psychological research.
[20] UTE LEONARDS,et al. The Influence of Temporal Phase Differences on Texture Segmentation , 1996, Vision Research.
[21] R Blake,et al. Spatial and temporal coherence in perceptual binding. , 1997, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[22] W. D. Ross,et al. A Neural Theory of Attentive Visual Search : Interactions of Boundary , Surface , Spatial , and Object Representations By : Stephen Grossberg , 2004 .
[23] Lynn C. Robertson,et al. Visual Search Performance in the Neglect Syndrome , 1989, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[24] K L Shapiro,et al. Temporary suppression of visual processing in an RSVP task: an attentional blink? . , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[25] John Duncan,et al. A neural basis for visual search in inferior temporal cortex , 1993, Nature.
[26] J. Duncan,et al. Systematic analysis of deficits in visual attention. , 1999 .
[27] M. Corbetta,et al. Superior Parietal Cortex Activation During Spatial Attention Shifts and Visual Feature Conjunction , 1995, Science.
[28] A Treisman,et al. Perceptual objects and the cost of filtering , 1983, Perception & psychophysics.
[29] A. Cowey,et al. Left visuo-spatial neglect can be worse in far than in near space , 1994, Neuropsychologia.
[30] Alan Kingstone,et al. Visual offsets facilitate saccadic latency: Does predisengagement of visuospatial attention mediate this gap effect? , 1993 .
[31] Derrick G. Watson,et al. Fractionating the preview benefit in search: dual-task decomposition of visual marking by timing and modality. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[32] S. Yantis,et al. Stimulus-driven attentional capture: evidence from equiluminant visual objects. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[33] C. Eriksen,et al. Temporal and spatial characteristics of selective encoding from visual displays , 1972 .
[34] D. Bub,et al. Modulation of the Directional Attention Deficit in Visual Neglect by Hemispatial Factors , 1993, Brain and Cognition.
[35] M. Posner,et al. Orienting of Attention* , 1980, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[36] M. Goldberg,et al. The representation of visual salience in monkey parietal cortex , 1998, Nature.
[37] B. Motter. Neural correlates of attentive selection for color or luminance in extrastriate area V4 , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.
[38] M. Jeannerod. Neurophysiological and neuropsychological aspects of spatial neglect. , 1987 .
[39] Bradley S. Gibson,et al. Stimulus-Driven Attentional Capture Is Contingent on Attentional Set for Displaywide Visual Features , 1998 .
[40] Glyn W. Humphreys,et al. The effect of cueing on unilateral neglect , 1983, Neuropsychologia.
[41] Glyn W. Humphreys,et al. Separating distractor rejection and target detection in posterior parietal cortex—an event-related fMRI study of visual marking , 2003, NeuroImage.
[42] Karl J. Friston,et al. The physiological basis of attentional modulation in extrastriate visual areas , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.
[43] J Theeuwes,et al. Visual marking beside the mark: Prioritizing selection by abrupt onsets , 2001, Perception & psychophysics.
[44] Edward K. Vogel,et al. The capacity of visual working memory for features and conjunctions , 1997, Nature.
[45] J. Marshall,et al. Left neglect for near but not far space in man , 1991, Nature.
[46] M. Farah,et al. Parietal lobe mechanisms of spatial attention: Modality-specific or supramodal? , 1989, Neuropsychologia.
[47] R. Walker,et al. Saccadic eye movement programming in unilateral neglect , 1996, Neuropsychologia.
[48] C. Kennard,et al. Abnormal temporal dynamics of visual attention in spatial neglect patients , 1997, Nature.
[49] R. Bruyer,et al. Ipsilateral influences on contralateral processing in neglect patients , 1989 .
[50] J. Duncan,et al. Competitive brain activity in visual attention , 1997, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.
[51] Olaf B. Paulson,et al. Parieto–Occipital Areas Involved in Efficient Filtering in Search: A Time Course Analysis of Visual Marking using Behavioural and Functional Imaging Procedures , 2004, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.
[52] A. Nobre,et al. Where and When to Pay Attention: The Neural Systems for Directing Attention to Spatial Locations and to Time Intervals as Revealed by Both PET and fMRI , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[53] Y Tsal,et al. Attending to color and shape: The special role of location in selective visual processing , 1988, Perception & psychophysics.
[54] Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al. Mechanisms of visual attention in the human cortex. , 2000, Annual review of neuroscience.
[55] Derrick G. Watson,et al. Visual marking: Evidence for inhibition using a probe-dot detection paradigm , 2000, Perception & psychophysics.
[56] M. Gado,et al. Functional Anatomy of the Cerebral Cortex by Computed Tomography , 1979, Journal of computer assisted tomography.
[57] J. Mattingley,et al. Visual extinction and prior entry: Impaired perception of temporal order with intact motion perception after unilateral parietal damage , 1997, Neuropsychologia.
[58] F. J. Friedrich,et al. Effects of parietal injury on covert orienting of attention , 1984, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.
[59] Glyn W. Humphreys,et al. Lesioning a connectionist model of visual search: selective effects on distractor grouping. , 1992, Canadian journal of psychology.
[60] Marius Usher,et al. Visual synchrony affects binding and segmentation in perception , 1998, Nature.
[61] Glyn W. Humphreys,et al. Visual marking: using time in visual selection , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[62] Melina A. Kunar,et al. History Matters , 2003, Psychological science.
[63] H Pashler,et al. Familiarity and visual change detection , 1988, Perception & psychophysics.
[64] Marvin M. Chun,et al. Visual marking: selective attention to asynchronous temporal groups. , 2002 .
[65] N. Kanwisher,et al. Visual attention: Insights from brain imaging , 2000, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[66] Glyn W. Humphreys,et al. Visual marking inhibits singleton capture , 2003, Cognitive Psychology.
[67] Stephen Smith,et al. Prioritizing new over old: An fMRI study of the preview search task , 2005, Human brain mapping.
[68] H. Ives. XII. Studies in the photometry of lights of different colours , 1912 .
[69] A. Treisman,et al. Parietal contributions to visual feature binding: evidence from a patient with bilateral lesions , 1995, Science.
[70] S. Yantis,et al. Mechanisms of attentional priority. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[71] Ian H. Robertson,et al. Unilateral Neglect: Clinical and Experimental Studies edited by Ian H. Robertson and John C. Marshall , 1994 .
[72] Glyn W. Humphreys,et al. Perceptual and Action Systems in Unilateral Visual Neglect , 1987 .
[73] A. Treisman,et al. A feature-integration theory of attention , 1980, Cognitive Psychology.
[74] G. Humphreys,et al. When visual marking meets the attentional blink: More evidence for top-down, limited capacity inhibition , 2002 .
[75] John H. R. Maunsell,et al. Attentional modulation of visual motion processing in cortical areas MT and MST , 1996, Nature.
[76] R. Rafal,et al. Shifting visual attention between objects and locations: evidence from normal and parietal lesion subjects. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. General.