The Psychophysics of Binocular Vision

This chapter reviews psychophysical research on human stereoscopic processes and their relationship to a 3D-TV system with DIBR. Topics include basic physiology, binocular correspondence and the horopter, stereoacuity and fusion limits, non-corresponding inputs and rivalry, dynamic cues to depth and their interactions with disparity, and development and adaptability of the binocular system.

[1]  I. P. Howard,et al.  Effects of stimulus size and eccentricity on horizontal and vertical vergence , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.

[2]  K. Nakayama,et al.  Real world occlusion constraints and binocular rivalry , 1990, Vision Research.

[3]  K. N. Ogle On the limits of stereoscopic vision. , 1952, Journal of experimental psychology.

[4]  George Mather,et al.  Foundations of Perception , 2006 .

[5]  S. Palmer Vision Science : Photons to Phenomenology , 1999 .

[6]  H. Helmholtz Helmholtz's Treatise on Physiological Optics , 1963 .

[7]  T Ledgeway,et al.  The Effects of Eccentricity and Vergence Angle upon the Relative Tilt of Corresponding Vertical and Horizontal Meridia Revealed Using the Minimum Motion Paradigm , 1999, Perception.

[8]  Laurie M. Wilcox,et al.  Viewing stereoscopic images comfortably: the effects of whole-field vertical disparity , 2002, IS&T/SPIE Electronic Imaging.

[9]  Alexander Toet,et al.  Visual comfort of binocular and 3D displays , 2004 .

[10]  Jari Takatalo,et al.  Evaluation of stereoscopic image quality for mobile devices using interpretation based quality methodology , 2009, Electronic Imaging.

[11]  Martin S Banks,et al.  The surface of the empirical horopter. , 2008, Journal of vision.

[12]  Robert Patterson,et al.  Immersive stereo displays, intuitive reasoning, and cognitive engineering , 2009 .

[13]  I. Howard Seeing in depth, Vol. 1: Basic mechanisms. , 2002 .

[14]  G. Westheimer,et al.  Effects of practice and the separation of test targets on foveal and peripheral stereoacuity , 1983, Vision Research.

[15]  M. J. Morgan,et al.  Stereoscopic depth perception at high velocities , 1995, Nature.

[16]  Alexander I. Cogan,et al.  The relationship between the apparent vertical and the vertical horopter , 1979, Vision Research.

[17]  P. Panum Physiologische Untersuchungen über das Sehen mit zwei Augen , 1858 .

[18]  Mtm Marc Lambooij,et al.  Visual Discomfort and Visual Fatigue of Stereoscopic Displays: A Review , 2009 .

[19]  W. J. Tam,et al.  Display duration and stereoscopic depth discrimination. , 1998, Canadian journal of experimental psychology = Revue canadienne de psychologie experimentale.

[20]  David M. Hoffman,et al.  Perceptual Issues in Stereoscopic Signal Processing , 2011, IEEE Transactions on Broadcasting.

[21]  Matthias Wöpking,et al.  Viewing comfort with stereoscopic pictures : an experimental study on the subjective effects of disparity magnitude and depth of focus , 1995 .

[22]  Makoto Okui,et al.  Geometrical analysis of puppet-theater and cardboard effects in stereoscopic HDTV images , 2006, IEEE Transactions on Circuits and Systems for Video Technology.

[23]  C. Schor,et al.  Binocular sensory fusion is limited by spatial resolution , 1984, Vision Research.

[24]  Filippo Speranza,et al.  Improving the visual comfort of stereoscopic images , 2003, IS&T/SPIE Electronic Imaging.

[25]  Susan R. Barry Fixing My Gaze: A Scientist's Journey Into Seeing in Three Dimensions , 2009 .

[26]  Scott B. Steinman,et al.  Foundations of Binocular Vision: A Clinical Perspective , 2000 .

[27]  D Humphriss,et al.  The Psychological Septum. An Investigation into Its Function , 1982, American journal of optometry and physiological optics.

[28]  Inna Tsirlin,et al.  Monocular occlusions determine the perceived shape and depth of occluding surfaces. , 2010, Journal of vision.

[29]  C. Curcio,et al.  Topography of ganglion cells in human retina , 1990, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[30]  Wijnand A. IJsselsteijn,et al.  A survey of perceptual evaluations and requirements of three-dimensional TV , 2004, IEEE Transactions on Circuits and Systems for Video Technology.

[31]  Philip M. Grove,et al.  Amodal completion with background determines depth from monocular gap stereopsis , 2006, Vision Research.

[32]  Ken Nakayama,et al.  Quantitative depth for a phantom surface can be based on cyclopean occlusion cues alone , 1999, Vision Research.

[33]  A. Tschermak,et al.  Fortgesetzte Studien Über Binokularsehen , 1924, Pflugers Archiv fur die gesamte Physiologie des Menschen und der Tiere.

[34]  H. Ono,et al.  Interocular Transfer of a Rotational Motion Aftereffect as a Function of Eccentricity , 2008, Perception.

[35]  C. Fehn,et al.  Interactive 3-DTV-Concepts and Key Technologies , 2006 .

[36]  Philip A Duke,et al.  Monocular transparency generates quantitative depth , 2003, Vision Research.

[37]  Laurie M. Wilcox,et al.  The role of monocularly visible regions in depth and surface perception , 2009, Vision Research.

[38]  D V Meegan,et al.  Unequal weighting of monocular inputs in binocular combination: implications for the compression of stereoscopic imagery. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Applied.

[39]  Yi-Zhong Wang,et al.  The Critical Period for Susceptibility of Human Stereopsis , 2005 .

[40]  Julie M. Harris Monocular zones in stereoscopic scenes: A useful source of information for human binocular vision? , 2010, Electronic Imaging.

[41]  S. Dumais,et al.  Stereopsis in human infants. , 1980, Science.

[42]  R. Held,et al.  Stereoacuity development for crossed and uncrossed disparities in human infants , 1982, Vision Research.

[43]  Lei Liu,et al.  Quantitative stereoscopic depth without binocular correspondence , 1994, Nature.

[44]  Barbara Gillam,et al.  Depth Interval Estimates from Motion Parallax and Binocular Disparity beyond Interaction Space , 2011, Perception.

[45]  T Shipley,et al.  The nonius horopter. II. An experimental report. , 1970, Vision research.

[46]  S. McKee,et al.  Steroscopic acuity for moving retinal images. , 1978, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[47]  D. Hubel,et al.  Receptive fields of single neurones in the cat's striate cortex , 1959, The Journal of physiology.

[48]  Katsumi Aoki,et al.  Recent development of flow visualization , 2004, J. Vis..

[49]  W. Levelt On binocular rivalry , 1965 .

[50]  Jin Liu Stereo image compression: the importance of spatial resolution in half-occluded regions , 1995, Electronic Imaging.

[51]  Hirokazu Yamanoue The Relation between Size Distortion and Shooting Conditions for Stereoscopic Images , 1997 .

[52]  Filippo Speranza,et al.  Stereoscopic 3D-TV: Visual Comfort , 2011, IEEE Transactions on Broadcasting.

[53]  Vieth Ueber die Richtung der Augen , 1818 .

[54]  H. Fukuyama,et al.  Functional anatomy on perception of position and motion in depth. , 1996, Neuroreport.

[55]  Robert S. Allison,et al.  Analysis of the influence of vertical disparities arising in Toed-in stereoscopic cameras , 2007 .

[56]  Laurie M Wilcox,et al.  Depth from Binocular Half-Occlusions in Stereoscopic Images of Natural Scenes , 2007, Perception.

[57]  Shinsuke Shimojo,et al.  Da vinci stereopsis: Depth and subjective occluding contours from unpaired image points , 1990, Vision Research.

[58]  T Shipley,et al.  Stereoscopic acuity and horizontal angular distance from fixation. , 1969, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[59]  I. Howard,et al.  Seeing in depth, Vol. 2: Depth perception. , 2002 .

[60]  Antonio Grieco Work with display units 94 , 2013 .

[61]  Carlos Vázquez,et al.  3D-TV Content Creation: Automatic 2D-to-3D Video Conversion , 2011, IEEE Transactions on Broadcasting.

[62]  G. Nyman,et al.  Occlusion Constraints and Stereoscopic Slant , 1997, Perception.

[64]  Takeo Iida,et al.  The effect of pupil size on accommodation , 1976, Vision Research.

[65]  Wijnand A. IJsselsteijn,et al.  Perceived quality of compressed stereoscopic images: Effects of symmetric and asymmetric JPEG coding and camera separation , 2006, TAP.

[66]  B. Julesz,et al.  A disparity gradient limit for binocular fusion. , 1980, Science.

[67]  Kenneth N. Ogle,et al.  Corresponding Retinal Points, the Horopter and Size and Shape of Ocular Images* , 1932 .

[68]  Joh. Müller Zur vergleichenden Physiologie des Gesichtssinnes des Menschen und der Thiere : nebst einem Versuch über die Bewegungen der Augen und über den menschlichen Blick , 1826 .

[69]  Bernard Mendiburu,et al.  3D Movie Making: Stereoscopic Digital Cinema from Script to Screen , 2009 .

[70]  H. Ono,et al.  The Effect of Stimulus Size on Stereoscopic Fusion Limits and Response Criteria , 2011, Perception.

[71]  Mel W. Siegel,et al.  Just enough reality: comfortable 3-D viewing via microstereopsis , 2000, IEEE Trans. Circuits Syst. Video Technol..

[72]  Hiroshi Ono,et al.  Content and context of monocular regions determine perceived depth in random dot, unpaired background and phantom stereograms , 2002, Vision Research.

[73]  B Gillam,et al.  The Role of Monocular Regions in Stereoscopic Displays , 1988, Perception.

[74]  Jonathan Freeman,et al.  Focus group exploration of presence through advanced broadcast services , 2000, Electronic Imaging.

[75]  Hiroyasu Ujike,et al.  Motion Parallax Driven by Head Movements: Conditions for Visual Stability, Perceived Depth, and Perceived Concomitant Motion , 2005, Perception.

[76]  Derek H. Arnold,et al.  Staying focused: a functional account of perceptual suppression during binocular rivalry. , 2007, Journal of vision.

[77]  Barton L. Anderson,et al.  The role of partial occlusion in stereopsis , 1994, Nature.

[78]  H Ono,et al.  The Backward Inclination of a Surface Defined by Empirical Corresponding Points , 2001, Perception.

[79]  Mark F. Bradshaw,et al.  Sensitivity to horizontal and vertical corrugations defined by binocular disparity , 1999, Vision Research.

[80]  Liang Zhang,et al.  Stereoscopic image generation based on depth images for 3D TV , 2005, IEEE Transactions on Broadcasting.

[81]  Nao Ninomiya,et al.  The 10th anniversary of journal of visualization , 2007, J. Vis..

[82]  Peter Lennie,et al.  Binocular integration of partially occluded surfaces , 2002, Vision Research.

[83]  H. Ono,et al.  Ecologically Invalid Monocular Texture Leads to Longer Perceptual Latencies in Random-Dot Stereograms , 1999, Perception.

[84]  R. Fox,et al.  The effect of testing method on stereoanomaly , 1984, Vision Research.

[85]  Philip M. Grove,et al.  Slant or occlusion: global factors resolve stereoscopic ambiguity in sets of horizontal lines , 2004, Vision Research.

[86]  Robert S. Allison,et al.  Sensitivity to Monocular Occlusions in Stereoscopic Imagery: Implications for S3D Content Creation, Distribution and Exhibition , 2010 .

[87]  M Ohmi,et al.  Dynamic Occlusion and Motion Parallax in Depth Perception , 1988, Perception.

[88]  Johannes Burge,et al.  The vertical horopter is not adaptable, but it may be adaptive. , 2011, Journal of vision.

[89]  Filippo Speranza,et al.  Effect of disparity and motion on visual comfort of stereoscopic images , 2006, Electronic Imaging.

[90]  C. Blakemore,et al.  The neural mechanism of binocular depth discrimination , 1967, The Journal of physiology.

[91]  J. Droulez,et al.  The visual perception of three-dimensional shape from self-motion and object-motion , 1994, Vision Research.

[92]  Fumio Okano,et al.  Measurement of parallax distribution and its application to the analysis of visual comfort for stereoscopic HDTV , 2003, IS&T/SPIE Electronic Imaging.

[93]  Hiroshi Ono,et al.  Leonardo's constraint: two opaque objects cannot be seen in the same direction. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[94]  K. Fujii,et al.  Visualization for the analysis of fluid motion , 2005, J. Vis..

[95]  P. Howarth The geometric horopter , 2011, Vision Research.

[96]  B. Gillam,et al.  How Configurations of Binocular Disparity Determine Whether Stereoscopic Slant or Stereoscopic Occlusion is Seen , 2005, Perception.

[97]  A. Kertesz,et al.  The effect of stimulus size on human cyclofusional response , 1978, Vision Research.

[98]  Sumio Yano,et al.  A study of visual fatigue and visual comfort for 3D HDTV/HDTV images , 2002 .

[99]  C. Blakemore The range and scope of binocular depth discrimination in man , 1970, The Journal of physiology.

[100]  Harold E Bedell,et al.  Stereopsis, cyclovergence and the backwards tilt of the vertical horopter , 1999, Vision Research.