Differences in cognitive performance during pregnancy and early motherhood

Background. Pregnancy has often been associated with cognitive deficits, but results are equivocal and little is known about how these deficits progress with time. Method. In the present study, the cognitive performance of 57 pregnant women was compared with that of 50 non-pregnant women matched for age and education, using a well-validated neurocognitive test battery at weeks 14, 17, 29, and 36 of pregnancy, and 32 weeks postpartum in the pregnant group and at comparable times in the non-pregnant group. Results. Memory encoding and retrieval, as assessed with a word learning task, were significantly lower in the pregnant group than in the control group. This difference was still present at 32 weeks after delivery. The two groups did not differ in complex speed of information processing at any of the test moments; however, general speed of information processing was significantly compromised during early motherhood (week 32 postpartum). Conclusion. Thus, memory performance is poorer during pregnancy and early motherhood, and general speed of information processing is slower during early motherhood.

[1]  G. Turner,et al.  Memory loss during pregnancy , 1993, British journal of obstetrics and gynaecology.

[2]  P. Casey,et al.  Memory in pregnancy. II: Implicit, incidental, explicit, semantic, short-term, working and prospective memory in primigravid, multigravid and postpartum women. , 1999, Journal of Psychosomatic Obstetrics and Gynaecology.

[3]  S. Baxendale,et al.  Motherhood and memory: a review , 2001, Psychoneuroendocrinology.

[4]  D. Fuerst,et al.  Explicit memory in pregnant women. , 1998, American journal of obstetrics and gynecology.

[5]  O. Almkvist,et al.  Temporary peripartal impairment in memory and attention and its possible relation to oxytocin concentration. , 1990, Life sciences.

[6]  L Svennerholm,et al.  Distribution and fatty acid composition of phosphoglycerides in normal human brain. , 1968, Journal of lipid research.

[7]  A. Eidelman,et al.  Cognitive deficits in women after childbirth. , 1993, Obstetrics and gynecology.

[8]  M. W. Brown,et al.  Objective and subjective memory impairment in pregnancy , 1991, Psychological Medicine.

[9]  P. Houx Cognitive aging and health-related factors , 1991 .

[10]  J. Jolles,et al.  Learning and retrieval rate of words presented auditorily and visually. , 1985, The Journal of general psychology.

[11]  J. Adam,et al.  Alpha-linolenic acid supplementation during human pregnancy does not effect cognitive functioning. , 2004, Prostaglandins, leukotrienes, and essential fatty acids.

[12]  A. Hofman,et al.  Periventricular cerebral white matter lesions predict rate of cognitive decline , 2002, Annals of neurology.

[13]  J. Jolles,et al.  Age-Related Decline of Psychomotor Speed: Effects of Age, Brain Health, Sex, and Education , 1993, Perceptual and motor skills.

[14]  B. Sherwin,et al.  Estrogen Use and Verbal Memory in Healthy Postmenopausal Women , 1994, Obstetrics and gynecology.

[15]  J. Jolles,et al.  Effect of test duration on age-related differences in Stroop interference. , 1997, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[16]  J. Ridley Studies of Interference in Serial Verbal Reactions , 2001 .

[17]  C. Currie,et al.  Indicators of socioeconomic status for adolescents: the WHO Health Behaviour in School-aged Children Survey. , 1997, Health education research.

[18]  S. Redman,et al.  Self-reported cognitive change during pregnancy. , 1991, The Australian journal of advanced nursing : a quarterly publication of the Royal Australian Nursing Federation.

[19]  J. Jolles,et al.  The relation between morbidity and cognitive performance in a normal aging population. , 1998, The journals of gerontology. Series A, Biological sciences and medical sciences.

[20]  G. Hornstra,et al.  Increased risk of postpartum depressive symptoms is associated with slower normalization after pregnancy of the functional docosahexaenoic acid status. , 2003, Prostaglandins, leukotrienes, and essential fatty acids.

[21]  N. Bohnen,et al.  Performance in the Stroop color word test in relationship to the persistence of symptoms following mild head injury , 1992, Acta neurologica Scandinavica.

[22]  W. Sturm,et al.  Neuropsychological assessment , 2007, Journal of Neurology.

[23]  Kathryn A. Lee,et al.  Sleep disturbances, vitality, and fatigue among a select group of employed childbearing women. , 1992, Birth.

[24]  P. Sastry,et al.  Lipids of nervous tissue: composition and metabolism. , 1985, Progress in lipid research.

[25]  A. Douglas,et al.  Brain preparations for maternity--adaptive changes in behavioral and neuroendocrine systems during pregnancy and lactation. An overview. , 2001, Progress in brain research.

[26]  C. Poser,et al.  Benign encephalopathy of pregnancy Preliminary clinical observations , 1986, Acta neurologica Scandinavica.

[27]  P. Martikainen,et al.  Pathways between socioeconomic determinants of health , 2004, Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health.

[28]  Elizabeth Hampson,et al.  Estrogen-related variations in human spatial and articulatory-motor skills , 1990, Psychoneuroendocrinology.

[29]  J. Yesavage,et al.  94285052 Estrogen replacement therapy and memory in older women , 1995 .

[30]  M. Rice,et al.  Postmenopausal estrogen and estrogen-progestin use and 2-year rate of cognitive change in a cohort of older Japanese American women: The Kame Project. , 2000, Archives of internal medicine.

[31]  I. Deary,et al.  Peripartal cognitive impairment: Secondary to depression? , 1996 .

[32]  J. Jolles,et al.  Long‐Term Persisting Cognitive Sequelae of Traumatic Brain Injury and the Effect of Age , 1996, The Journal of nervous and mental disease.

[33]  D. Kimura,et al.  Reciprocal effects of hormonal fluctuations on human motor and perceptual-spatial skills. , 1988, Behavioral neuroscience.

[34]  J. Jolles,et al.  Cognitive functioning in young and middle-aged unmedicated out-patients with major depression: testing the effort and cognitive speed hypotheses , 2003, Psychological Medicine.

[35]  Kathryn A. Lee,et al.  Longitudinal changes in fatigue and energy during pregnancy and the postpartum period. , 1999, Journal of obstetric, gynecologic, and neonatal nursing : JOGNN.

[36]  G. Hornstra,et al.  Comparison of the peripartum and postpartum phospholipid polyunsaturated fatty acid profiles of lactating and nonlactating women. , 2001, The American journal of clinical nutrition.

[37]  Frank Z. Stanczyk,et al.  Pregnancy, the postpartum, and steroid hormones: effects on cognition and mood , 1999, Psychoneuroendocrinology.

[38]  T. Hasaart,et al.  Maternal essential fatty acid patterns during normal pregnancy and their relationship to the neonatal essential fatty acid status , 1995, British Journal of Nutrition.

[39]  N Morris,et al.  Mood and cognition in pregnant workers. , 1998, Applied ergonomics.

[40]  P. Pollitt,et al.  Pregnancy may confer a selective cognitive advantage , 1999 .

[41]  J. Jolles,et al.  Information processing in depression and anxiety , 1987, Psychological Medicine.