Virtual Progress: The Effect of Path Characteristics on Perceptions of Progress and Choice

In goal-oriented services, consumers want to get transported from one well-defined state (start) to another (destination) state without much concern for intermediate states. A cost-based evaluation of such services should depend on the total cost associated with the service--i.e., the price and the amount of time taken for completion. In this paper, we demonstrate that the characteristics of the path to the final destination also influence evaluation and choice. Specifically, we show that segments of idle time and travel away from the final destination are seen as obstacles in the progress towards the destination, and hence lower the choice likelihood of the path. Further, we show that the earlier such obstacles occur during the service, the lower is the choice likelihood. We present an analytical model of consumer choice and test its predictions in a series of experiments. Our results show that in choosing between two services that cover the same displacement in the same time (i.e., identical average progress), consumer choice is driven by the perception of progress towards the goal (i.e., byvirtual progress). In a final experiment, we show that the effects of virtual progress may outweigh the effects of actual average progress.

[1]  Joel Huber,et al.  The Impact of Anticipating Satisfaction on Consumer Choice , 2000 .

[2]  A. Tversky,et al.  Prospect Theory : An Analysis of Decision under Risk Author ( s ) : , 2007 .

[3]  Jack M. Feldman,et al.  Self-generated validity and other effects of measurement on belief, attitude, intention, and behavior. , 1988 .

[4]  E. A. Locke,et al.  A theory of goal setting & task performance , 1990 .

[5]  R. Thaler Mental accounting matters , 1999 .

[6]  Bruce G. S. Hardie,et al.  Modeling Loss Aversion and Reference Dependence Effects on Brand Choice , 1993 .

[7]  G. Loewenstein Out of control: Visceral influences on behavior , 1996 .

[8]  D. Kahneman,et al.  Duration neglect in retrospective evaluations of affective episodes. , 1993, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[9]  K. Sheldon,et al.  Coherence and congruence: two aspects of personality integration. , 1995, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[10]  D. Ariely Combining experiences over time: the effects of duration, intensity changes and on‐line measurements on retrospective pain evaluations , 1998 .

[11]  J. Elster,et al.  Utility from memory and anticipation. , 1992 .

[12]  Y. Trope,et al.  The role of feasibility and desirability considerations in near and distant future decisions: A test of temporal construal theory. , 1998 .

[13]  John F. Kihlstrom,et al.  Personality and social intelligence , 1987 .

[14]  J. Elster,et al.  Choice Over Time , 1992 .

[15]  D. Ariely,et al.  Gestalt characteristics of experiences: the defining features of summarized events , 2000 .

[16]  pale self-regarding,et al.  The Psychology of Adjustment , 1937, Mental Welfare.

[17]  A. Tversky,et al.  Prospect theory: analysis of decision under risk , 1979 .

[18]  Kennon M. Sheldon,et al.  Pursuing Personal Goals: Skills Enable Progress, but Not all Progress is Beneficial , 1998 .

[19]  Dilip Soman,et al.  Prospective and retrospective evaluations of experiences: How you evaluate an experience depends on when you evaluate it , 2003 .

[20]  J. George Shanthikumar,et al.  A Psychological Perspective on Service Segmentation Models: The Significance of Accounting for Consumers' Perceptions of Waiting and Service , 1995 .

[21]  Ray W. Cooksey,et al.  Judgment analysis : theory, methods, and applications , 1996 .

[22]  A. Tversky,et al.  Loss Aversion in Riskless Choice: A Reference-Dependent Model , 1991 .

[23]  Shirley Taylor,et al.  The effects of filled waiting time and service provider control over the delay on evaluations of service , 1995 .

[24]  D. Kahneman,et al.  When More Pain Is Preferred to Less: Adding a Better End , 1993 .

[25]  Robert P. Abelson,et al.  Velocity relation : satisfaction as a function of the first derivative of outcome over time , 1991 .

[26]  D. Soman The mental accounting of sunk time costs: Why time is not like money. , 2001 .

[27]  G. Loewenstein,et al.  Anomalies in Intertemporal Choice: Evidence and an Interpretation , 1992 .

[28]  Peter Salovey,et al.  The Quasi-Acceleration Relation: Satisfaction as a Function of the Change of Velocity of Outcome Over Time , 2008 .

[29]  G. Loewenstein,et al.  Do Workers Prefer Increasing Wage Profiles? , 1991, Journal of Labor Economics.

[30]  C. W. Telford,et al.  Psychology of adjustment , 1967 .

[31]  J. Brunstein,et al.  Personal goals and subjective well-being: A longitudinal study. , 1993 .

[32]  Richard C. Larson,et al.  OR Forum - Perspectives on Queues: Social Justice and the Psychology of Queueing , 1987, Oper. Res..

[33]  Edward L. Deci,et al.  Intrinsic Motivation and Self-Determination in Human Behavior , 1975, Perspectives in Social Psychology.

[34]  W B Allen,et al.  The Logistics Revolution and Transportation , 1997 .

[35]  E B White Outcomes: essential information for clinical decision support: an interview with Ellen B. White. Interview by Melinda L. Orlando. , 1998, Journal of health care finance.

[36]  D. Soman The Illusion of Delayed Incentives: Evaluating Future Effort–Money Transactions , 1998 .

[37]  D. A. Kenny,et al.  The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. , 1986, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[38]  Bernd H. Schmitt,et al.  Consumers' Reactions to Waiting: When Delays Affect the Perception of Service Quality , 1989 .

[39]  Shirley Taylor Waiting for Service: The Relationship between Delays and Evaluations of Service , 1994 .

[40]  Drazen Prelec,et al.  Preferences for sequences of outcomes. , 1993 .

[41]  E. E. Osuna The psychological cost of waiting , 1985 .