The oblique effect is both allocentric and egocentric.

Despite continuous movements of the head, humans maintain a stable representation of the visual world, which seems to remain always upright. The mechanisms behind this stability are largely unknown. To gain some insight on how head tilt affects visual perception, we investigate whether a well-known orientation-dependent visual phenomenon, the oblique effect-superior performance for stimuli at cardinal orientations (0° and 90°) compared with oblique orientations (45°)-is anchored in egocentric or allocentric coordinates. To this aim, we measured orientation discrimination thresholds at various orientations for different head positions both in body upright and in supine positions. We report that, in the body upright position, the oblique effect remains anchored in allocentric coordinates irrespective of head position. When lying supine, gravitational effects in the plane orthogonal to gravity are discounted. Under these conditions, the oblique effect was less marked than when upright, and anchored in egocentric coordinates. The results are well explained by a simple "compulsory fusion" model in which the head-based and the gravity-based signals are combined with different weightings (30% and 70%, respectively), even when this leads to reduced sensitivity in orientation discrimination.

[1]  R. K. Simpson Nature Neuroscience , 2022 .

[2]  Guy Cheron,et al.  Two Reference Frames for Visual Perception in Two Gravity Conditions , 2005, Perception.

[3]  S. Appelle Perception and discrimination as a function of stimulus orientation: the "oblique effect" in man and animals. , 1972, Psychological bulletin.

[4]  F. Mast Human perception of verticality: Psychophysical experiments on the centrifuge and their neuronal implications , 2000 .

[5]  Jeremy Freeman,et al.  Orientation Decoding Depends on Maps, Not Columns , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[6]  David C. Burr,et al.  Orientation discrimination depends on spatial frequency , 1991, Vision Research.

[7]  D W Heeley,et al.  Anisotropic Axes in Orientation Perception are Not Retinotopically Mapped , 1993, Perception.

[8]  Eero P. Simoncelli,et al.  Cardinal rules: Visual orientation perception reflects knowledge of environmental statistics , 2011, Nature Neuroscience.

[9]  Wim Vanduffel,et al.  The Radial Bias: A Different Slant on Visual Orientation Sensitivity in Human and Nonhuman Primates , 2006, Neuron.

[10]  H. H. Kornhuber,et al.  Multisensory Convergence on Cortical Neurons Neuronal Effects of Visual, Acoustic and Vestibular Stimuli in the Superior Convolutions of the Cat's Cortex , 1963 .

[11]  Colin W. G. Clifford,et al.  Discrimination of the local orientation structure of spiral Glass patterns early in human visual cortex , 2009, NeuroImage.

[12]  R. Hill,et al.  Modifications of Receptive Fields of Cells in the Visual Cortex occurring Spontaneously and associated with Bodily Tilt , 1969, Nature.

[13]  R. Freeman,et al.  Oblique effect: a neural basis in the visual cortex. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[14]  G. Orban,et al.  Human orientation discrimination tested with long stimuli , 1984, Vision Research.

[15]  R. Wurtz Neuronal mechanisms of visual stability , 2008, Vision Research.

[16]  Maria Concetta Morrone,et al.  Constructing Stable Spatial Maps of the Word , 2012, Perception.

[17]  H. Mittelstaedt The subjective vertical as a function of visual and extraretinal cues. , 1986, Acta psychologica.

[18]  Gang Wang,et al.  Difference in the representation of cardinal and oblique contours in cat visual cortex , 2003, Neuroscience Letters.

[19]  M. Landy,et al.  Ideal-Observer Models of Cue Integration , 2012 .

[20]  C. Furmanski,et al.  An oblique effect in human primary visual cortex , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[21]  J. S. Wright,et al.  The oblique effect in orientation acuity , 1997, Vision Research.

[22]  D. Straumann,et al.  Single motor unit activity in human extraocular muscles during the vestibulo‐ocular reflex , 2012, The Journal of physiology.

[23]  D. Hubel,et al.  Receptive fields of single neurones in the cat's striate cortex , 1959, The Journal of physiology.

[24]  John H. R. Maunsell,et al.  Coding of image contrast in central visual pathways of the macaque monkey , 1990, Vision Research.

[25]  James M. Hillis,et al.  Combining Sensory Information: Mandatory Fusion Within, but Not Between, Senses , 2002, Science.

[26]  Jonathan W. Peirce,et al.  PsychoPy—Psychophysics software in Python , 2007, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[27]  Guy A. Orban,et al.  The neuronal machinery involved in successive orientation discrimination , 1998, Progress in Neurobiology.

[28]  M. Banks,et al.  The effect of head tilt on meridional differences in acuity: Implications for orientation constancy , 1975 .

[29]  Heinrich H. Bülthoff,et al.  Perceived Object Stability Depends on Multisensory Estimates of Gravity , 2011, PloS one.

[30]  Laurence R Harris,et al.  Shape-from-Shading Depends on Visual, Gravitational, and Body-Orientation Cues , 2004, Perception.

[31]  Duane Denney,et al.  Orientation specificity of visual cortical neurons after head tilt , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[32]  M Lipshits,et al.  Gravity affects the preferred vertical and horizontal in visual perception of orientation. , 1999, Neuroreport.

[33]  Michel Guerraz,et al.  Reference frames and haptic perception of orientation: Body and head tilt effects on the oblique effect , 2001, Perception & psychophysics.

[34]  Guang Bin Liu,et al.  Orientation mosaic in barn owl’s visual Wulst revealed by optical imaging: comparison with cat and monkey striate and extra-striate areas , 2003, Brain Research.

[35]  Jonathan Westley Peirce,et al.  Neuroinformatics Original Research Article Generating Stimuli for Neuroscience Using Psychopy , 2022 .

[36]  Edouard Gentaz,et al.  Body tilt effect on the reproduction of orientations: studies on the visual oblique effect and subjective orientations. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[37]  Thomas Haslwanter,et al.  Three-dimensional eye position during static roll and pitch in humans , 2001, Vision Research.

[38]  L. Harris,et al.  The subjective visual vertical and the perceptual upright , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.