The effect of auditory and visual distracters on the useful field of view: implications for the driving task.

PURPOSE The driving environment is becoming increasingly complex, including both visual and auditory distractions within the in-vehicle and external driving environments. This study was designed to investigate the effect of visual and auditory distractions on a performance measure that has been shown to be related to driving safety, the useful field of view. METHODS A laboratory study recorded the useful field of view in 28 young visually normal adults (mean 22.6 +/- 2.2 years). The useful field of view was measured in the presence and absence of visual distracters (of the same angular subtense as the target) and with three levels of auditory distraction (none, listening only, listening and responding). RESULTS Central errors increased significantly (P < 0.05) in the presence of auditory but not visual distracters, while peripheral errors increased in the presence of both visual and auditory distracters. Peripheral errors increased with eccentricity and were greatest in the inferior region in the presence of distracters. CONCLUSIONS Visual and auditory distracters reduce the extent of the useful field of view, and these effects are exacerbated in inferior and peripheral locations. This result has significant ramifications for road safety in an increasingly complex in-vehicle and driving environment.

[1]  K. Ball,et al.  Vision impairment, eye disease, and injurious motor vehicle crashes in the elderly. , 1998, Ophthalmic epidemiology.

[2]  D. E. Irwin,et al.  Conversation Disrupts Visual Scanning of Traffic Scenes , 2001 .

[3]  D. Roenker,et al.  Age and visual search: expanding the useful field of view. , 1988, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[4]  D. Tellinghuisen,et al.  The inability to ignore auditory distractors as a function of visual task perceptual load , 2003, Perception & psychophysics.

[5]  J. Katz,et al.  Handbook of clinical audiology , 1978 .

[6]  J. Szlyk,et al.  Age-related functional field losses are not eccentricity dependent , 1996, Vision Research.

[7]  Beatriz Muñoz,et al.  Measures of Visual Function and Time to Driving Cessation in Older Adults , 2005, Optometry and vision science : official publication of the American Academy of Optometry.

[8]  F. Cornelissen,et al.  Age-related changes in the functional visual field: further evidence for an inverse Age x Eccentricity effect. , 2004, The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences.

[9]  C. Scialfa,et al.  Age differences in the useful field of view: an eye movement analysis. , 1994, Optometry and vision science : official publication of the American Academy of Optometry.

[10]  M. Sloane,et al.  Visual processing impairment and risk of motor vehicle crash among older adults. , 1998, JAMA.

[11]  R. Sekuler,et al.  Visual localization: age and practice. , 1986, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[12]  Miyoshi Ayama,et al.  Effects of visual and conversation tasks upon a functional visual field while driving , 2000, ITSC2000. 2000 IEEE Intelligent Transportation Systems. Proceedings (Cat. No.00TH8493).

[13]  D. G. Payne,et al.  Effects of Speech Intelligibility Level on Concurrent Visual Task Performance , 1994, Human factors.

[14]  Moshe Eizenman,et al.  THE IMPACT OF COGNITIVE DISTRACTION ON DRIVER VISUAL BEHAVIOUR AND VEHICLE CONTROL , 2002 .

[15]  K. Ball,et al.  Visual attention problems as a predictor of vehicle crashes in older drivers. , 1993, Investigative ophthalmology & visual science.

[16]  David Zadok,et al.  Visual field attention is reduced by concomitant hands-free conversation on a cellular telephone. , 2004, American journal of ophthalmology.

[17]  M. D’Esposito Working memory. , 2008, Handbook of clinical neurology.

[18]  Min-Shik Kim,et al.  Visual Search Does Not Remain Efficient When Executive Working Memory Is Working , 2004, Psychological science.

[19]  Giovanni Galfano,et al.  Nonspatial attentional shifts between audition and vision. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[20]  Mitsuo Ikeda,et al.  Influence of foveal load on the functional visual field , 1975 .

[21]  D. C. Howell Statistical Methods for Psychology , 1987 .

[22]  A. Baddeley Is working memory still working? , 2001, The American psychologist.

[23]  David L. Strayer,et al.  Driven to Distraction: Dual-Task Studies of Simulated Driving and Conversing on a Cellular Telephone , 2001, Psychological science.

[24]  D. C. Howell Statistical methods for psychology, 3rd ed. , 1992 .

[25]  J. Wood,et al.  Age and Visual Impairment Decrease Driving Performance as Measured on a Closed-Road Circuit , 2002, Hum. Factors.

[26]  David E. Irwin,et al.  Conversation Disrupts Change Detection in Complex Traffic Scenes , 2004, Hum. Factors.

[27]  Paul Atchley,et al.  Conversation Limits the Functional Field of View , 2004, Hum. Factors.

[28]  J. Wood,et al.  The effect of artificial visual impairment on functional visual fields and driving performance , 1993 .

[29]  N. Mackworth Visual noise causes tunnel vision , 1965 .

[30]  D. Strayer,et al.  Cell phone-induced failures of visual attention during simulated driving. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Applied.

[31]  B. Munoz,et al.  Association of Visual Field Loss and Mobility Performance in Older Adults: Salisbury Eye Evaluation Study , 2004, Optometry and vision science : official publication of the American Academy of Optometry.

[32]  M. Pomplun,et al.  Investigating the visual span in comparative search: the effects of task difficulty and divided attention , 2001, Cognition.

[33]  I. Brown,et al.  Interference between concurrent tasks of driving and telephoning. , 1969, The Journal of applied psychology.