Orientation toward humans predicts cognitive performance in orang-utans
暂无分享,去创建一个
Judith M Burkart | J. Call | B. Galdikas | K. Liebal | D. Haun | C. V. van Schaik | E. Willems | S. Forss | Z. Kosonen | L. Damerius
[1] Carel P. van Schaik,et al. Observational social learning and socially induced practice of routine skills in immature wild orang-utans , 2016, Animal Behaviour.
[2] Josep Call,et al. Cognitive differences between orang-utan species: a test of the cultural intelligence hypothesis , 2016, Scientific Reports.
[3] C. Hemelrijk,et al. Early maternal loss affects social integration of chimpanzees throughout their lifetime , 2015, Scientific Reports.
[4] Nicole Zweifel,et al. Contrasting responses to novelty by wild and captive orangutans , 2015, American journal of primatology.
[5] Judith M Burkart,et al. High emotional reactivity toward an experimenter affects participation, but not performance, in cognitive tests with common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus) , 2015, Animal Cognition.
[6] A. S. Griffin,et al. Innovation and problem solving: A review of common mechanisms , 2014, Behavioural Processes.
[7] S. R. Ross,et al. Distributed under Creative Commons Cc-by 4.0 the Impact of Atypical Early Histories on Pet or Performer Chimpanzees , 2022 .
[8] D. Bates,et al. Fitting Linear Mixed-Effects Models Using lme4 , 2014, 1406.5823.
[9] Roger Bakeman,et al. Emotional engagements predict and enhance social cognition in young chimpanzees , 2014, Developmental science.
[10] E. van de Waal,et al. Potent Social Learning and Conformity Shape a Wild Primate’s Foraging Decisions , 2013, Science.
[11] Sarah Benson-Amram,et al. A comparison of innovative problem-solving abilities between wild and captive spotted hyaenas, Crocuta crocuta , 2013, Animal Behaviour.
[12] Shinichi Nakagawa,et al. A general and simple method for obtaining R2 from generalized linear mixed‐effects models , 2013 .
[13] L. Lefebvre,et al. Problem-solving performance is correlated with reproductive success in a wild bird population , 2013, Animal Behaviour.
[14] Sarah Benson-Amram,et al. Innovative problem solving by wild spotted hyenas , 2012, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[15] Josep Call,et al. Are there geniuses among the apes? , 2012, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[16] A. S. Griffin,et al. Consumer and motor innovation in the common myna: the role of motivation and emotional responses , 2012, Animal Behaviour.
[17] J. Flynn,et al. Intelligence: new findings and theoretical developments. , 2012, The American psychologist.
[18] M. Tomasello,et al. A comparison of temperament in nonhuman apes and human infants. , 2011, Developmental science.
[19] Stephan R Lehner,et al. Can captive orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus abelii) be coaxed into cumulative build-up of techniques? , 2011, Journal of comparative psychology.
[20] A. Kacelnik,et al. Flexibility in Problem Solving and Tool Use of Kea and New Caledonian Crows in a Multi Access Box Paradigm , 2011, PloS one.
[21] Michael Tomasello,et al. Differences in the Cognitive Skills of Bonobos and Chimpanzees , 2010, PloS one.
[22] R. Bshary,et al. Selective attention to philopatric models causes directed social learning in wild vervet monkeys , 2010, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[23] S. Wich,et al. Social learning of diet and foraging skills by wild immature Bornean orangutans: implications for culture , 2010, American journal of primatology.
[24] Satoshi Hirata,et al. How to crack nuts: acquisition process in captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) observing a model , 2009, Animal Cognition.
[25] T. Matsuzawa,et al. Social influences on ant-dipping acquisition in the wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes verus) of Bossou, Guinea, West Africa , 2009, Animal Cognition.
[26] M. E. Laidre,et al. Spontaneous performance of wild baboons on three novel food-access puzzles , 2008, Animal Cognition.
[27] Andrew Whiten,et al. Observational learning from tool using models by human-reared and mother-reared capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) , 2008, Animal Cognition.
[28] S. Boysen,et al. Raking it in: the impact of enculturation on chimpanzee tool use , 2007, Animal Cognition.
[29] D. Bjorklund. Mother knows best: Epigenetic inheritance, maternal effects, and the evolution of human intelligence , 2006 .
[30] Laurie R Santos,et al. Probing the limits of tool competence: Experiments with two non-tool-using species (Cercopithecus aethiops and Saguinus oedipus) , 2006, Animal Cognition.
[31] M. Tomasello,et al. The role of humans in the cognitive development of apes revisited , 2004, Animal Cognition.
[32] J. Bering. A critical review of the “enculturation hypothesis”: the effects of human rearing on great ape social cognition , 2004, Animal Cognition.
[33] W. Gilmer,et al. Early experience and depressive disorders: human and non-human primate studies. , 2003, Journal of affective disorders.
[34] E. Visalberghi,et al. Response Toward Novel Foods and Novel Objects in Wild Cebus apella , 2003, International Journal of Primatology.
[35] S. Obayashi,et al. Rapid learning of sequential tool use by macaque monkeys , 2003, Physiology & Behavior.
[36] A. Fuentes. The Mentalities of Gorillas and Orangutans: Comparative Perspectives , 2001 .
[37] Michael Tomasello,et al. The Cultural Ecology of Young Children’s Interactions With Objects and Artifacts , 1999 .
[38] R. Byrne,et al. Priming primates: Human and otherwise , 1998, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[39] S. Shelton,et al. Asymmetric frontal brain activity, cortisol, and behavior associated with fearful temperament in rhesus monkeys. , 1998, Behavioral neuroscience.
[40] K. Bard,et al. Influences on development in infant chimpanzees: enculturation temperament, and cognition , 1996 .
[41] M. Tomasello,et al. Imitative learning of actions on objects by children, chimpanzees, and enculturated chimpanzees. , 1993, Child development.
[42] A. Russon,et al. Imitation in free-ranging rehabilitant orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus). , 1993, Journal of comparative psychology.
[43] N L Segal,et al. “Sources of Human Psychological Differences: The Minnesota Study of Twins Reared Apart” (1990), by , 2018 .
[44] S. Shelton,et al. Defensive behaviors in infant rhesus monkeys: environmental cues and neurochemical regulation. , 1989, Science.
[45] B. Beck. Animal Tool Behavior: The Use and Manufacture of Tools by Animals , 1980 .
[46] D. Rumbaugh,et al. Long-Term Cognitive Deficits in Chimpanzees Associated with Early Impoverished Rearing. , 1973 .
[47] H. Harlow,et al. Differential effect of early enrichment and deprivation on learning in the rhesus monkey (Macaca mulatta). , 1973 .
[48] C. M. Rogers,et al. The effect of early deprivation on the social behavior of adolescent chimpanzees. , 1969, The American journal of psychiatry.
[49] H. Harlow,et al. Affectional responses in the infant monkey; orphaned baby monkeys develop a strong and persistent attachment to inanimate surrogate mothers. , 1959, Science.
[50] R. Yerkes. Ideational Behavior of Monkeys and Apes. , 1916, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[51] T. Werner. Intelligence Of Apes And Other Rational Beings , 2016 .
[52] V. Bentley-Condit,et al. Animal tool use: current definitions and an updated comprehensive catalog , 2010 .
[53] K. Bard,et al. Socio-emotional factors in the development of joint attention in human and ape infants. , 2009 .
[54] E. Lonsdorf. What is the role of mothers in the acquisition of termite-fishing behaviors in wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii)? , 2005, Animal Cognition.
[55] Michael Tomasello,et al. The effect of humans on the cognitive development of apes , 1996 .
[56] R. Sternberg,et al. Intelligence: Knowns and unknowns. , 1996 .
[57] H. Harlow,et al. The formation of learning sets. , 1949, Psychological review.