Slime mold uses an externalized spatial “memory” to navigate in complex environments

Spatial memory enhances an organism’s navigational ability. Memory typically resides within the brain, but what if an organism has no brain? We show that the brainless slime mold Physarum polycephalum constructs a form of spatial memory by avoiding areas it has previously explored. This mechanism allows the slime mold to solve the U-shaped trap problem—a classic test of autonomous navigational ability commonly used in robotics—requiring the slime mold to reach a chemoattractive goal behind a U-shaped barrier. Drawn into the trap, the organism must rely on other methods than gradient-following to escape and reach the goal. Our data show that spatial memory enhances the organism’s ability to navigate in complex environments. We provide a unique demonstration of a spatial memory system in a nonneuronal organism, supporting the theory that an externalized spatial memory may be the functional precursor to the internal memory of higher organisms.

[1]  H. P. Rusch,et al.  Isolation and Characterization of an Extracellular Polysaccharide from Physarum polycephalum , 1970, Journal of bacteriology.

[2]  The function of slime from Physarum flavicomum in the control of cell division. , 1975, Canadian journal of microbiology.

[3]  E. B. Ridgway,et al.  Control of chemotaxis in Physarum polycephalum , 1976, The Journal of cell biology.

[4]  M. Carlile,et al.  The chemotactic response of plasmodia of the myxomycete Physarum polycephalum to sugars and related compounds. , 1978, Journal of general microbiology.

[5]  T. Mansour,et al.  Chemotaxis toward carbohydrates and amino acids in Physarum polycephalum. , 1978, Experimental cell research.

[6]  T. Mansour,et al.  Measurement of chemotaxis in the slime mold Physarum polycephalum. , 1978, Experimental cell research.

[7]  T. Ueda,et al.  Membrane biophysics of chemoreception and taxis in the plasmodium of Physarum polycephalum. , 1980, Biophysical chemistry.

[8]  C. L. Gass,et al.  SHOULD FORAGERS REMEMBER WHERE THEY'VE BEEN? EXPLORATIONS OF A SIMULATION MODEL BASED ON THE BEHAVIOR AND ENERGETICS OF TERRITORIAL HUMMINGBIRDS , 1987 .

[9]  Tucker R. Balch,et al.  Avoiding the past: a simple but effective strategy for reactive navigation , 1993, [1993] Proceedings IEEE International Conference on Robotics and Automation.

[10]  Ronald C. Arkin,et al.  Temporal coordination of perceptual algorithms for mobile robot navigation , 1994, IEEE Trans. Robotics Autom..

[11]  C. Bernstein,et al.  Patch-marking and optimal search patterns in the parasitoid Venturia canescens , 1996 .

[12]  Dyer,et al.  Spatial memory and navigation by honeybees on the scale of the foraging range , 1996, The Journal of experimental biology.

[13]  R. Wehner,et al.  Visual navigation in insects: coupling of egocentric and geocentric information , 1996, The Journal of experimental biology.

[14]  T. Nakagaki,et al.  Intelligence: Maze-solving by an amoeboid organism , 2000, Nature.

[15]  Fumitoshi Matsuno,et al.  Use of single side reflex for autonomous navigation of mobile robots in unknown environments , 2001, Robotics Auton. Syst..

[16]  T. Nakagaki,et al.  Path finding by tube morphogenesis in an amoeboid organism. , 2001, Biophysical chemistry.

[17]  Roger D. Quinn,et al.  Toward Mission Capable Legged Robots through Biological Inspiration , 2001, Auton. Robots.

[18]  A. E. Kilian,et al.  Life-like learning in technical artefacts: biochemical vs. neuronal mechanisms , 2002, Proceedings of the 9th International Conference on Neural Information Processing, 2002. ICONIP '02..

[19]  Ronald C. Arkin,et al.  Line-of-sight constrained exploration for reactive multiagent robotic teams , 2002, 7th International Workshop on Advanced Motion Control. Proceedings (Cat. No.02TH8623).

[20]  Lynne E. Parker,et al.  Guest editorial advances in multirobot systems , 2002, IEEE Trans. Robotics Autom..

[21]  邹细勇,et al.  Virtual local target method for avoiding local minimum in potential field based robot navigation , 2003 .

[22]  T. Nakagaki,et al.  Smart network solutions in an amoeboid organism. , 2004, Biophysical chemistry.

[23]  Y. Nishiura,et al.  Obtaining multiple separate food sources: behavioural intelligence in the Physarum plasmodium , 2004, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[24]  J. Zeil Orientation flights of solitary wasps (Cerceris; Sphecidae; Hymenoptera) , 1993, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[25]  F. Ratnieks,et al.  Longevity and detection of persistent foraging trails in Pharaoh's ants, Monomorium pharaonis (L.) , 2006, Animal Behaviour.

[26]  Marco Dorigo,et al.  Self-organisation and communication in groups of simulated and physical robots , 2006, Biological Cybernetics.

[27]  R. Wehner,et al.  The Ant Odometer: Stepping on Stilts and Stumps , 2006, Science.

[28]  A. Tero,et al.  Minimum-risk path finding by an adaptive amoebal network. , 2007, Physical review letters.

[29]  Guan-Chun Luh,et al.  An immunological approach to mobile robot reactive navigation , 2008, Appl. Soft Comput..

[30]  Yoonsuck Choe,et al.  Evolution of recollection and prediction in neural networks , 2009, 2009 International Joint Conference on Neural Networks.

[31]  I. Couzin Collective cognition in animal groups , 2009, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[32]  Yoonsuck Choe,et al.  Emergence of Memory-like Behavior in Reactive Agents Using External Markers , 2009, 2009 21st IEEE International Conference on Tools with Artificial Intelligence.

[33]  M. Beekman,et al.  Irrational decision-making in an amoeboid organism: transitivity and context-dependent preferences , 2011, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[34]  Ws. Rasband ImageJ, U.S. National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, Maryland, USA , 2011 .

[35]  P. Graham,et al.  What can we learn from studies of insect navigation? , 2012, Animal Behaviour.