Bracing for the Psychological Storm: Proactive versus Reactive Compensatory Consumption

This research introduces the distinction between compensatory consumption that is engaged in after, as opposed to before, one experiences a self-threat (termed reactive vs. proactive compensatory consumption). Five experiments document the phenomenon of proactive compensatory consumption as well as corresponding boundary conditions for its effect. Furthermore, whereas both reactive and proactive compensatory consumption are associated with seeking products that symbolically relate to an experienced or potential threat, we demonstrate that reactive compensatory consumption is more likely to be associated with the use of products for the purpose of distraction. We examine how and when these different forms of compensatory consumption affect consumers' preferences versus actual consumption behavior. Implications for delineating reactive versus proactive compensatory consumption in the literature, as well as the use of consumption for the purpose of symbolic self-completion versus distraction, are discussed.

[1]  K. Keith Heine, Steven J , 2013 .

[2]  R. Petty,et al.  The elaboration likelihood model , 2012 .

[3]  Keisha M. Cutright,et al.  When Your World Must Be Defended: Choosing Products to Justify the System , 2011 .

[4]  Susan M. Broniarczyk,et al.  It’s Not Me, It’s You: How Gift Giving Creates Giver Identity Threat as a Function of Social Closeness , 2011 .

[5]  K. Vohs,et al.  Social Exclusion Causes People to Spend and Consume Strategically in the Service of Affiliation , 2011 .

[6]  Niro Sivanathan,et al.  Protecting the self through consumption: Status goods as affirmational commodities , 2010 .

[7]  Rui Zhu,et al.  Seeking Freedom through Variety , 2009 .

[8]  S. C. Wheeler,et al.  The “Shaken Self”: Product Choices as a Means of Restoring Self‐View Confidence , 2009 .

[9]  Derek D. Rucker,et al.  Desire to Acquire: Powerlessness and Compensatory Consumption , 2008 .

[10]  N. Mandel,et al.  The Sweet Escape: Effects of Mortality Salience on Consumption Quantities for High- and Low-Self-Esteem Consumers , 2008 .

[11]  J. Boldero,et al.  Self-Lines: A New, Psychometrically Sound, ‘User-Friendly’ Idiographic Technique for Assessing Self-Discrepancies , 2006, Cognitive Therapy and Research.

[12]  Kathleen D. Vohs,et al.  The Meaning Maintenance Model: On the Coherence of Social Motivations , 2006, Personality and social psychology review : an official journal of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Inc.

[13]  Michael Johns,et al.  Combating stereotype threat: The effect of self-affirmation on women's intellectual performance. , 2006 .

[14]  P. Cramer Protecting the self , 2006 .

[15]  James R. Bettman,et al.  Let Us Eat and Drink, for Tomorrow We Shall Die: Effects of Mortality Salience and Self-Esteem on Self-Regulation in Consumer Choice , 2005 .

[16]  J. Shah The Automatic Pursuit and Management of Goals , 2005 .

[17]  David Laibson,et al.  Experienced Utility and Objective Happiness: A Moment-Based Approach , 2001 .

[18]  D. Kahneman,et al.  Back to Bentham? Explorations of experience utility , 1997 .

[19]  Shelley E. Taylor,et al.  Social cognition, 2nd ed. , 1991 .

[20]  P. Nedungadi Recall and Consumer Consideration Sets: Influencing Choice without Altering Brand Evaluations , 1990 .

[21]  C. Steele The Psychology of Self-Affirmation: Sustaining the Integrity of the Self , 1988 .

[22]  E. Higgins,et al.  Self-discrepancy: a theory relating self and affect. , 1987, Psychological review.

[23]  John T. Cacioppo,et al.  The Elaboration Likelihood Model of Persuasion , 1986, Advances in Experimental Social Psychology.

[24]  J. Hornik Subjective vs. Objective Time Measures: A Note on the Perception of Time in Consumer Behavior , 1984 .

[25]  Peter M. Gollwitzer,et al.  Symbolic self-completion , 1982 .