Using a symbolic process model as input for model-based fMRI analysis: Locating the neural correlates of problem state replacements

In this paper, a model-based analysis method for fMRI is used with a high-level symbolic process model. Participants performed a triple-task in which intermediate task information needs to be updated frequently. Previous work has shown that the associated resource - the problem state resource - acts as a bottleneck in multitasking. The model-based method was used to locate the neural correlates of 'problem state replacements'. To analyze the fMRI data, we fit the computational process model to the behavioral data and regressed the model's activity against the fMRI data. The brain region responsible for the temporary representation of problem states, the inferior parietal lobule, and the brain region responsible for long-term storage of problem states, the inferior frontal gyrus were thus identified. These results show that model-based fMRI analyses can be performed using high-level symbolic cognitive models, enabling fine-grained exploratory fMRI research.

[1]  Klaus Wunderlich,et al.  Neural computations underlying action-based decision making in the human brain , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[2]  E E Smith,et al.  Components of verbal working memory: evidence from neuroimaging. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[3]  John R. Anderson,et al.  A central circuit of the mind , 2008, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[4]  Florin Dolcos,et al.  Similarities and Differences in the Neural Correlates of Episodic Memory Retrieval and Working Memory , 2002, NeuroImage.

[5]  J. O'Doherty,et al.  The Role of the Ventromedial Prefrontal Cortex in Abstract State-Based Inference during Decision Making in Humans , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[6]  John R. Anderson How Can the Human Mind Occur in the Physical Universe , 2007 .

[7]  Niels Taatgen,et al.  The Multitasking Mind , 2010, Oxford series on cognitive models and architectures.

[8]  J. O'Doherty,et al.  Is Avoiding an Aversive Outcome Rewarding? Neural Substrates of Avoidance Learning in the Human Brain , 2006, PLoS biology.

[9]  D. Gitelman,et al.  Neuroanatomic Overlap of Working Memory and Spatial Attention Networks: A Functional MRI Comparison within Subjects , 1999, NeuroImage.

[10]  R. Baayen,et al.  Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items , 2008 .

[11]  N. Taatgen,et al.  The problem state: a cognitive bottleneck in multitasking. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[12]  R. Henson,et al.  Frontal lobes and human memory: insights from functional neuroimaging. , 2001, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[13]  D. Schacter,et al.  Prefrontal Contributions to Executive Control: fMRI Evidence for Functional Distinctions within Lateral Prefrontal Cortex , 2001, NeuroImage.

[14]  Mitsuo Kawato,et al.  Heterarchical reinforcement-learning model for integration of multiple cortico-striatal loops: fMRI examination in stimulus-action-reward association learning , 2006, Neural Networks.

[15]  N. Logothetis What we can do and what we cannot do with fMRI , 2008, Nature.

[16]  B McElree,et al.  Working memory and focal attention. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[17]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Nonlinear Regression in Parametric Activation Studies , 1996, NeuroImage.

[18]  R. Cabeza,et al.  Imaging Cognition II: An Empirical Review of 275 PET and fMRI Studies , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[19]  John R. Anderson,et al.  Endogenous Control and Task Representation: An fMRI Study in Algebraic Problem-solving , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[20]  Edward E. Smith,et al.  Neuroimaging studies of working memory: , 2003, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[21]  A. Newell Unified Theories of Cognition , 1990 .

[22]  J. O'Doherty,et al.  Model‐Based fMRI and Its Application to Reward Learning and Decision Making , 2007, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[23]  John R. Anderson,et al.  An information-processing model of the BOLD response in symbol manipulation tasks , 2003, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[24]  Mark S. Cohen,et al.  Parametric Analysis of fMRI Data Using Linear Systems Methods , 1997, NeuroImage.

[25]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Statistical parametric mapping , 2013 .

[26]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Statistical parametric mapping , 2013 .

[27]  Jelmer P. Borst,et al.  The Costs of Multitasking in Threaded Cognition , 2007 .

[28]  M. Hogge,et al.  Exploration of the neural substrates of executive functioning by functional neuroimaging , 2006, Neuroscience.

[29]  John P O'Doherty,et al.  Model-based approaches to neuroimaging: combining reinforcement learning theory with fMRI data. , 2010, Wiley interdisciplinary reviews. Cognitive science.

[30]  Niels Taatgen,et al.  Toward a unified theory of the multitasking continuum: from concurrent performance to task switching, interruption, and resumption , 2009, CHI.

[31]  F. Collette,et al.  Brain imaging of the central executive component of working memory , 2002, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[32]  Dario D. Salvucci,et al.  Threaded cognition: an integrated theory of concurrent multitasking. , 2008, Psychological review.

[33]  Andrea Stocco,et al.  The Neural Correlates of Problem States: Testing fMRI Predictions of a Computational Model of Multitasking , 2010, PloS one.

[34]  Myeong-Ho Sohn,et al.  An information-processing model of three cortical regions: evidence in episodic memory retrieval , 2005, NeuroImage.

[35]  John R. Anderson,et al.  Human Symbol Manipulation Within an Integrated Cognitive Architecture , 2005, Cogn. Sci..

[36]  John R. Anderson,et al.  Reflections of the Environment in Memory Form of the Memory Functions , 2022 .

[37]  Hugh Garavan,et al.  Serial attention within working memory , 1998, Memory & cognition.

[38]  Richard P. Cooper,et al.  The Role of Falsification in the Development of Cognitive Architectures: Insights from a Lakatosian Analysis , 2007, Cogn. Sci..

[39]  John R. Anderson,et al.  Tracing Problem Solving in Real Time: fMRI Analysis of the Subject-paced Tower of Hanoi , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[40]  P. Dayan,et al.  Cortical substrates for exploratory decisions in humans , 2006, Nature.

[41]  I. J. Myung,et al.  When a good fit can be bad , 2002, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.