Exploring the modulation of attentional capture by attentional control settings using performance and illusory line motion

Modulation of attentional capture by attentional control settings was explored using performance and phenomenology. Trials began with four figure-8s presented above, below, left, and right of fixation. Any figure-8 (or none) brightened uninformatively (cue) before presentation of either a digit target (2 or 5 made from a figure-8 by deletion) calling for a speeded identification, or a line connecting adjacent figure-8s calling for a motion judgement. Such lines are seen drawn away from an adjacent cue. Digit targets appeared only on the horizontal or vertical axes, encouraging voluntary attention to two (target-relevant) of the four figure-8s. Whereas voluntary attention reduced attentional capture from cues at taskirrelevant locations (when lines were unlikely to be presented near the task-irrelevant locations), it had no effect on motion judgements, suggesting that cue-elicited exogenous attention affects perceptual arrival times while voluntary attention prioritizes locations for further processing without affecting arrival times.

[1]  M. Schmitter-Edgecombe,et al.  Costs of a predictable switch between simple cognitive tasks following severe closed-head injury. , 2006, Neuropsychology.

[2]  R. Klein,et al.  Does attention cause illusory line motion? , 2005, Perception & psychophysics.

[3]  F. Chavane,et al.  Imaging cortical correlates of illusion in early visual cortex , 2004, Nature.

[4]  D. Bavelier,et al.  Components of visual prior entry , 2003, Cognitive Psychology.

[5]  R. Klein,et al.  Does attention follow the motion in the “shooting line” illusion? , 2002, Perception & psychophysics.

[6]  B. Dosher,et al.  Spatial attention: different mechanisms for central and peripheral temporal precues? , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[7]  W. Schmidt,et al.  Endogenous attention and illusory line motion reexamined. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[8]  H. Pashler,et al.  Evidence for split attentional foci. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[9]  S. Watamaniuk High-Level Motion Processing: Computational, Neurobiological, and Psychophysical Perspectives , 1999 .

[10]  George R. Mangun,et al.  Reflexive Attention Modulates Processing of Visual Stimuli in Human Extrastriate Cortex , 1998, Psychological science.

[11]  R. Remington,et al.  Selectivity in distraction by irrelevant featural singletons: evidence for two forms of attentional capture. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[12]  R. Klein,et al.  A Spatial Gradient of Acceleration and Temporal Extension Underlies Three Illusions of Motion , 1997, Perception.

[13]  A. Treisman,et al.  The line-motion illusion: attention or impletion? , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[14]  L. Optican,et al.  Involuntary attentional shifts due to orientation differences , 1996, Perception & psychophysics.

[15]  Jocelyn Faubert,et al.  The influence of two spatially distinct primers and attribute priming on motion induction , 1995, Vision Research.

[16]  S. Monsell,et al.  Costs of a predictible switch between simple cognitive tasks. , 1995 .

[17]  J. Faubert,et al.  Processing Speed in the Motion-Induction Effect , 1995, Perception.

[18]  J. Juola,et al.  Tradeoffs between attentional effects of spatial cues and abrupt onsets , 1995, Perception & psychophysics.

[19]  S. Yantis,et al.  Stimulus-driven attentional capture: evidence from equiluminant visual objects. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[20]  O. Hikosaka,et al.  Focal visual attention produces illusory temporal order and motion sensation , 1993, Vision Research.

[21]  O. Hikosaka,et al.  Voluntary and Stimulus-Induced Attention Detected as Motion Sensation , 1993, Perception.

[22]  J. C. Johnston,et al.  Involuntary covert orienting is contingent on attentional control settings. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[23]  L. Stelmach,et al.  Directed attention and perception of temporal order. , 1991, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[24]  J. Theeuwes Cross-dimensional perceptual selectivity , 1991, Perception & psychophysics.

[25]  S. Yantis,et al.  Abrupt visual onsets and selective attention: voluntary versus automatic allocation. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[26]  P. Rabbitt,et al.  Reflexive and voluntary orienting of visual attention: time course of activation and resistance to interruption , 1989 .

[27]  S. Yantis,et al.  Uniqueness of abrupt visual onset in capturing attention , 1988, Perception & psychophysics.

[28]  J. Findlay,et al.  Sensitivity and criterion effects in the spatial cuing of visual attention , 1987, Perception & psychophysics.

[29]  S. Yantis,et al.  Abrupt visual onsets and selective attention: evidence from visual search. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[30]  M. Posner,et al.  Neural systems control of spatial orienting. , 1982, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[31]  HighWire Press Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London , 1781, The London Medical Journal.

[32]  Cathy Frey,et al.  Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science , 2010 .

[33]  K. Schneider On visual prior entry , 2001 .

[34]  Richard B. Ivry,et al.  Task switching and multitask performance. , 2000 .

[35]  J. Theeuwes,et al.  On the time course of top-down and bottom-up control of visual attention , 2000 .

[36]  Lawrence M. Ward,et al.  An event-related brain potential study of inhibition of return , 1999, Perception & psychophysics.

[37]  Takeo Watanabe,et al.  The role of parsing in high level motion processing , 1998 .

[38]  J. Theeuwes Exogenous and endogenous control of attention: The effect of visual onsets and offsets , 1991, Perception & psychophysics.

[39]  H. J. Muller,et al.  Reflexive and voluntary orienting of visual attention: time course of activation and resistance to interruption. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[40]  G. Shulman,et al.  Spatial determinants of the distribution of attention , 1985, Perception & psychophysics.

[41]  M. Posner,et al.  Components of visual orienting , 1984 .

[42]  J. Duncan,et al.  Directing attention in the visual field , 1981, Perception & psychophysics.

[43]  J. Jonides Voluntary versus automatic control over the mind's eye's movement , 1981 .

[44]  Michael I. Posner,et al.  Please Scroll down for Article the Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Orienting of Attention Orienting of Attention* , 2022 .