The counting Stroop: a cognitive interference task

The counting Stroop is a validated Stroop task variant. Initially designed as a functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) task for identifying brain regions subserving cognition and attention (dorsal anterior midcingulate cortex (daMCC) and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC)), it has been used to study cognition in healthy volunteers and to identify functional brain abnormalities in neuropsychiatric disorders, such as attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). During the counting Stroop, subjects report by button-press the number of words (one to four) appearing on the screen, regardless of word meaning. Neutral-word control trials contain single semantic category common animals (e.g., 'dog' written three times), while interference trials contain number words that are incongruent with the correct response (e.g., 'two' written four times). The counting Stroop can be completed in approximately 20 min per subject and can be used offline (behavioral performance) or with fMRI, positron emission tomography, event-related potentials, magnetoencephalography or intracranial recordings.

[1]  R. C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[2]  J R Simon,et al.  Effect of conflicting cues on information processing: the 'Stroop effect' vs. the 'Simon effect'. , 1990, Acta psychologica.

[3]  M. Raichle,et al.  The anterior cingulate cortex mediates processing selection in the Stroop attentional conflict paradigm. , 1990, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[4]  Colin M. Macleod Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: an integrative review. , 1991, Psychological bulletin.

[5]  J. Stroop Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. , 1992 .

[6]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Investigations of the functional anatomy of attention using the stroop test , 1993, Neuropsychologia.

[7]  B. J. Casey,et al.  Regional brain activity when selecting a response despite interference: An H2 15O PET study of the stroop and an emotional stroop , 1994, Human brain mapping.

[8]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Interference and Facilitation Effects during Selective Attention: An H2 15O PET Study of Stroop Task Performance , 1995, NeuroImage.

[9]  T. Salthouse,et al.  Aging, inhibition, working memory, and speed. , 1995, The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences.

[10]  A M Dale,et al.  Randomized event‐related experimental designs allow for extremely rapid presentation rates using functional MRI , 1998, Neuroreport.

[11]  George Bush,et al.  The emotional counting stroop paradigm: a functional magnetic resonance imaging probe of the anterior cingulate affective division , 1998, Biological Psychiatry.

[12]  C. Umilta,et al.  Symbolic distance between numerosity and identity modulates Stroop interference. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[13]  S. Rauch,et al.  The counting stroop: An interference task specialized for functional neuroimaging—validation study with functional MRI , 1998, Human brain mapping.

[14]  Jenn-Yeu Chen Stroop Interference is the Result of Comparable, Not of Differential Processing Speeds of Two Stimulus Dimensions , 1998, Perceptual and motor skills.

[15]  S. Rauch,et al.  Anterior cingulate cortex dysfunction in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder revealed by fMRI and the counting stroop , 1999, Biological Psychiatry.

[16]  M. Posner,et al.  Cognitive and emotional influences in anterior cingulate cortex , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[17]  A. Dale,et al.  Dorsal anterior cingulate cortex: A role in reward-based decision making , 2001, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[18]  S. Clare,et al.  Imaging how attention modulates pain in humans using functional MRI. , 2002, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[19]  Allan L. Reiss,et al.  fMRI Study of Cognitive Interference Processing in Females with Fragile X Syndrome , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[20]  Stephen Smith,et al.  Potentially adaptive functional changes in cognitive processing for patients with multiple sclerosis and their acute modulation by rivastigmine. , 2003, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[21]  B R Rosen,et al.  The Multi-Source Interference Task: validation study with fMRI in individual subjects , 2003, Molecular Psychiatry.

[22]  Guy M. Goodwin,et al.  The role of the anterior cingulate cortex in the counting Stroop task , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[23]  Martin P Paulus,et al.  Functional subdivisions within anterior cingulate cortex and their relationship to autonomic nervous system function , 2004, NeuroImage.

[24]  Mark J Lowe,et al.  Media Violence Exposure and Frontal Lobe Activation Measured by Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging in Aggressive and Nonaggressive Adolescents , 2005, Journal of computer assisted tomography.

[25]  Michael S. Gaffrey,et al.  Activity and functional connectivity of inferior frontal cortex associated with response conflict. , 2005, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[26]  S. Holland,et al.  Abnormal FMRI brain activation in euthymic bipolar disorder patients during a counting Stroop interference task. , 2005, The American journal of psychiatry.

[27]  Nicholas Wymbs,et al.  Neural correlates of conflict processing , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.

[28]  Erich O. Richter,et al.  Human Anterior Cingulate Cortex Neurons Encode Cognitive and Emotional Demands , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[29]  G. Bush,et al.  The Multi-Source Interference Task: an fMRI task that reliably activates the cingulo-frontal-parietal cognitive/attention network , 2006, Nature Protocols.

[30]  George Bush,et al.  The emotional counting Stroop: a task for assessing emotional interference during brain imaging , 2006, Nature Protocols.

[31]  B. Mensour,et al.  Effect of neurofeedback training on the neural substrates of selective attention in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study , 2006, Neuroscience Letters.