Neural predictors of moment-to-moment fluctuations in cognitive flexibility

Cognitive flexibility is a crucial human ability allowing efficient adaptation to changing task challenges. Although a person's degree of flexibility can vary from moment to moment, the conditions regulating such fluctuations are not well understood. Using a task-switching procedure with fMRI, we found several brain regions in which neural activity preceding each trial predicted subsequent cognitive flexibility. Specifically, as pretrial activity increased, performance improved on trials when the task switched but did not improve when the task repeated. Regions from which flexibility could be predicted reliably included the basal ganglia, anterior cingulate cortex, prefrontal cortex, and posterior parietal cortex. Although further analysis revealed similarities across the regions in how flexibility was predicted, results supported the existence of multiple independent sources of prediction. These results reveal distinct neural mechanisms underlying fluctuations in cognitive flexibility.

[1]  M. M. Burns,et al.  Parkinsonism , 1975, Neurology.

[2]  M. Horstink,et al.  Cognitive and motor shifting aptitude disorder in Parkinson's disease. , 1984, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[3]  J. Talairach,et al.  Co-Planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain: 3-Dimensional Proportional System: An Approach to Cerebral Imaging , 1988 .

[4]  D. Alan Allport,et al.  SHIFTING INTENTIONAL SET - EXPLORING THE DYNAMIC CONTROL OF TASKS , 1994 .

[5]  S. Monsell,et al.  Costs of a predictible switch between simple cognitive tasks. , 1995 .

[6]  N. Meiran Reconfiguration of processing mode prior to task performance. , 1996 .

[7]  D H Brainard,et al.  The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[8]  D G Pelli,et al.  The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[9]  S Makeig,et al.  Analysis of fMRI data by blind separation into independent spatial components , 1998, Human brain mapping.

[10]  Aapo Hyvärinen,et al.  Fast and robust fixed-point algorithms for independent component analysis , 1999, IEEE Trans. Neural Networks.

[11]  R De Jong,et al.  An intention-activation account of residual switch costs , 2000 .

[12]  J. Cohen,et al.  Dissociating the role of the dorsolateral prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortex in cognitive control. , 2000, Science.

[13]  John R. Anderson,et al.  The role of prefrontal cortex and posterior parietal cortex in task switching. , 2000, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[14]  T. Robbins,et al.  Differential effects of 6-OHDA lesions of the frontal cortex and caudate nucleus on the ability to acquire an attentional set. , 2001, Cerebral cortex.

[15]  E. Miller,et al.  An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. , 2001, Annual review of neuroscience.

[16]  T. Robbins,et al.  Mechanisms of cognitive set flexibility in Parkinson's disease. , 2001, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[17]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Separating Processes within a Trial in Event-Related Functional MRI II. Analysis , 2001, NeuroImage.

[18]  Stephen Monsell,et al.  Residual costs in task switching: Testing the failure-to-engage hypothesis , 2002, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[19]  Jeremy R. Reynolds,et al.  Neural Mechanisms of Transient and Sustained Cognitive Control during Task Switching , 2003, Neuron.

[20]  Stephen Monsell,et al.  Task-Set Switching Deficits in Early-Stage Huntington's Disease: Implications for Basal Ganglia Function , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[21]  E. Miller,et al.  Neural circuits subserving the retrieval and maintenance of abstract rules. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[22]  R. Passingham,et al.  Prefrontal interactions reflect future task operations , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.

[23]  M. Brass,et al.  Decomposing Components of Task Preparation with Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[24]  T. Robbins,et al.  Impaired set-shifting and dissociable effects on tests of spatial working memory following the dopamine D2 receptor antagonist sulpiride in human volunteers , 2004, Psychopharmacology.

[25]  T. Robbins,et al.  Differential Responses in Human Striatum and Prefrontal Cortex to Changes in Object and Rule Relevance , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[26]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Anterior Cingulate Conflict Monitoring and Adjustments in Control , 2004, Science.

[27]  T. Robbins,et al.  Chemistry of the adaptive mind , 2004, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series A: Mathematical, Physical and Engineering Sciences.

[28]  K. Lesch,et al.  Dopamine and cognitive control: the influence of spontaneous eyeblink rate and dopamine gene polymorphisms on perseveration and distractibility. , 2005, Behavioral neuroscience.

[29]  Maurizio Corbetta,et al.  The human brain is intrinsically organized into dynamic, anticorrelated functional networks. , 2005, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[30]  Rainer Goebel,et al.  Analysis of functional image analysis contest (FIAC) data with brainvoyager QX: From single‐subject to cortically aligned group general linear model analysis and self‐organizing group independent component analysis , 2006, Human brain mapping.

[31]  M. Rugg,et al.  Brain activity before an event predicts later recollection , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.

[32]  E. Crone,et al.  Neural evidence for dissociable components of task-switching. , 2006, Cerebral cortex.

[33]  Ritske de Jong,et al.  Pre-stimulus EEG effects related to response speed, task switching and upcoming response hand , 2006, Biological Psychology.

[34]  Michael J. Frank,et al.  A mechanistic account of striatal dopamine function in human cognition: psychopharmacological studies with cabergoline and haloperidol. , 2006, Behavioral neuroscience.

[35]  Benjamin J. Shannon,et al.  Coherent spontaneous activity identifies a hippocampal-parietal memory network. , 2006, Journal of neurophysiology.

[36]  M. Chun,et al.  Linking Implicit and Explicit Memory: Common Encoding Factors and Shared Representations , 2006, Neuron.

[37]  Kristina M. Visscher,et al.  A Core System for the Implementation of Task Sets , 2006, Neuron.

[38]  Kristina M. Visscher,et al.  The neural bases of momentary lapses in attention , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.

[39]  Katsuyuki Sakai,et al.  Prefrontal Set Activity Predicts Rule-Specific Neural Processing during Subsequent Cognitive Performance , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[40]  R. Passingham,et al.  Reading Hidden Intentions in the Human Brain , 2007, Current Biology.

[41]  Justin L. Vincent,et al.  Intrinsic Fluctuations within Cortical Systems Account for Intertrial Variability in Human Behavior , 2007, Neuron.

[42]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  On the Control of Control: The Role of Dopamine in Regulating Prefrontal Function and Working Memory , 2007 .