Neural predictors of moment-to-moment fluctuations in cognitive flexibility
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] M. M. Burns,et al. Parkinsonism , 1975, Neurology.
[2] M. Horstink,et al. Cognitive and motor shifting aptitude disorder in Parkinson's disease. , 1984, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.
[3] J. Talairach,et al. Co-Planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain: 3-Dimensional Proportional System: An Approach to Cerebral Imaging , 1988 .
[4] D. Alan Allport,et al. SHIFTING INTENTIONAL SET - EXPLORING THE DYNAMIC CONTROL OF TASKS , 1994 .
[5] S. Monsell,et al. Costs of a predictible switch between simple cognitive tasks. , 1995 .
[6] N. Meiran. Reconfiguration of processing mode prior to task performance. , 1996 .
[7] D H Brainard,et al. The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.
[8] D G Pelli,et al. The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. , 1997, Spatial vision.
[9] S Makeig,et al. Analysis of fMRI data by blind separation into independent spatial components , 1998, Human brain mapping.
[10] Aapo Hyvärinen,et al. Fast and robust fixed-point algorithms for independent component analysis , 1999, IEEE Trans. Neural Networks.
[11] R De Jong,et al. An intention-activation account of residual switch costs , 2000 .
[12] J. Cohen,et al. Dissociating the role of the dorsolateral prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortex in cognitive control. , 2000, Science.
[13] John R. Anderson,et al. The role of prefrontal cortex and posterior parietal cortex in task switching. , 2000, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[14] T. Robbins,et al. Differential effects of 6-OHDA lesions of the frontal cortex and caudate nucleus on the ability to acquire an attentional set. , 2001, Cerebral cortex.
[15] E. Miller,et al. An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. , 2001, Annual review of neuroscience.
[16] T. Robbins,et al. Mechanisms of cognitive set flexibility in Parkinson's disease. , 2001, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[17] M. Corbetta,et al. Separating Processes within a Trial in Event-Related Functional MRI II. Analysis , 2001, NeuroImage.
[18] Stephen Monsell,et al. Residual costs in task switching: Testing the failure-to-engage hypothesis , 2002, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[19] Jeremy R. Reynolds,et al. Neural Mechanisms of Transient and Sustained Cognitive Control during Task Switching , 2003, Neuron.
[20] Stephen Monsell,et al. Task-Set Switching Deficits in Early-Stage Huntington's Disease: Implications for Basal Ganglia Function , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[21] E. Miller,et al. Neural circuits subserving the retrieval and maintenance of abstract rules. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.
[22] R. Passingham,et al. Prefrontal interactions reflect future task operations , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.
[23] M. Brass,et al. Decomposing Components of Task Preparation with Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[24] T. Robbins,et al. Impaired set-shifting and dissociable effects on tests of spatial working memory following the dopamine D2 receptor antagonist sulpiride in human volunteers , 2004, Psychopharmacology.
[25] T. Robbins,et al. Differential Responses in Human Striatum and Prefrontal Cortex to Changes in Object and Rule Relevance , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[26] Jonathan D. Cohen,et al. Anterior Cingulate Conflict Monitoring and Adjustments in Control , 2004, Science.
[27] T. Robbins,et al. Chemistry of the adaptive mind , 2004, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series A: Mathematical, Physical and Engineering Sciences.
[28] K. Lesch,et al. Dopamine and cognitive control: the influence of spontaneous eyeblink rate and dopamine gene polymorphisms on perseveration and distractibility. , 2005, Behavioral neuroscience.
[29] Maurizio Corbetta,et al. The human brain is intrinsically organized into dynamic, anticorrelated functional networks. , 2005, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[30] Rainer Goebel,et al. Analysis of functional image analysis contest (FIAC) data with brainvoyager QX: From single‐subject to cortically aligned group general linear model analysis and self‐organizing group independent component analysis , 2006, Human brain mapping.
[31] M. Rugg,et al. Brain activity before an event predicts later recollection , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.
[32] E. Crone,et al. Neural evidence for dissociable components of task-switching. , 2006, Cerebral cortex.
[33] Ritske de Jong,et al. Pre-stimulus EEG effects related to response speed, task switching and upcoming response hand , 2006, Biological Psychology.
[34] Michael J. Frank,et al. A mechanistic account of striatal dopamine function in human cognition: psychopharmacological studies with cabergoline and haloperidol. , 2006, Behavioral neuroscience.
[35] Benjamin J. Shannon,et al. Coherent spontaneous activity identifies a hippocampal-parietal memory network. , 2006, Journal of neurophysiology.
[36] M. Chun,et al. Linking Implicit and Explicit Memory: Common Encoding Factors and Shared Representations , 2006, Neuron.
[37] Kristina M. Visscher,et al. A Core System for the Implementation of Task Sets , 2006, Neuron.
[38] Kristina M. Visscher,et al. The neural bases of momentary lapses in attention , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.
[39] Katsuyuki Sakai,et al. Prefrontal Set Activity Predicts Rule-Specific Neural Processing during Subsequent Cognitive Performance , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[40] R. Passingham,et al. Reading Hidden Intentions in the Human Brain , 2007, Current Biology.
[41] Justin L. Vincent,et al. Intrinsic Fluctuations within Cortical Systems Account for Intertrial Variability in Human Behavior , 2007, Neuron.
[42] Jonathan D. Cohen,et al. On the Control of Control: The Role of Dopamine in Regulating Prefrontal Function and Working Memory , 2007 .