Speech, stone tool-making and the evolution of language

The ‘technological hypothesis’ proposes that gestural language evolved in early hominins to enable the cultural transmission of stone tool-making skills, with speech appearing later in response to the complex lithic industries of more recent hominins. However, no flintknapping study has assessed the efficiency of speech alone (unassisted by gesture) as a tool-making transmission aid. Here we show that subjects instructed by speech alone underperform in stone tool-making experiments in comparison to subjects instructed through either gesture alone or ‘full language’ (gesture plus speech), and also report lower satisfaction with their received instruction. The results provide evidence that gesture was likely to be selected over speech as a teaching aid in the earliest hominin tool-makers; that speech could not have replaced gesturing as a tool-making teaching aid in later hominins, possibly explaining the functional retention of gesturing in the full language of modern humans; and that speech may have evolved for reasons unrelated to tool-making. We conclude that speech is unlikely to have evolved as tool-making teaching aid superior to gesture, as claimed by the technological hypothesis, and therefore alternative views should be considered. For example, gestural language may have evolved to enable tool-making in earlier hominins, while speech may have later emerged as a response to increased trade and more complex inter- and intra-group interactions in Middle Pleistocene ancestors of Neanderthals and Homo sapiens; or gesture and speech may have evolved in parallel rather than in sequence.

[1]  William C. McCormack,et al.  Directions in cognitive anthropology , 1988 .

[2]  W. Fitch,et al.  Monkey vocal tracts are speech-ready , 2016, Science Advances.

[3]  D. McNeill So you think gestures are nonverbal , 1985 .

[4]  W. Strange Evolution of language. , 1984, JAMA.

[5]  Nick Blegen The earliest long-distance obsidian transport: Evidence from the ∼200 ka Middle Stone Age Sibilo School Road Site, Baringo, Kenya. , 2017, Journal of human evolution.

[6]  T. Chaminade,et al.  Stone tools, language and the brain in human evolution , 2012, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[7]  Noam Chomsky,et al.  How Could Language Have Evolved? , 2014, PLoS biology.

[8]  S. Goldin-Meadow,et al.  Why people gesture when they speak , 1998, Nature.

[9]  Robin I. M. Dunbar Grooming, Gossip and the Evolution of Language , 1996 .

[10]  A. Montagu TOOLMAKING, HUNTING, AND THE ORIGIN OF LANGUAGE , 1976 .

[11]  G. Hewes Primate Communication and the Gestural Origin of Language , 1992, Current Anthropology.

[12]  R. Holloway, Culture: A Human Domain , 1969, Current Anthropology.

[13]  H. Burbano,et al.  The Derived FOXP2 Variant of Modern Humans Was Shared with Neandertals , 2007, Current Biology.

[14]  A. Monaco,et al.  A forkhead-domain gene is mutated in a severe speech and language disorder , 2001, Nature.

[15]  Frederick L. Coolidge,et al.  Working Memory, its Executive Functions, and the Emergence of Modern Thinking , 2005 .

[16]  Shelby S. Putt,et al.  THE ROLE OF VERBAL INTERACTION DURING EXPERIMENTAL BIFACIAL STONE TOOL MANUFACTURE , 2014 .

[17]  K. Laland,et al.  Experimental Evidence for the Co-Evolution of Hominin Tool-Making Teaching and Language , 2014, Nature Communications.

[18]  R. Milo,et al.  Tools, language and cognition in human evolution , 1995, International Journal of Primatology.

[19]  Errett Callahan,et al.  The basics of biface knapping in the Eastern Fluted Tradition: a manual for flintknappers and lithic analysts (preface) , 1996 .

[20]  M. Arbib From monkey-like action recognition to human language: An evolutionary framework for neurolinguistics , 2005, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[21]  Kevin Fiedler,et al.  Grooming Gossip And The Evolution Of Language , 2016 .

[22]  M. Tomasello Origins of human communication , 2008 .

[23]  K. Aoki,et al.  Transmission of Tool-making through Verbal and Non-verbal Commu-nication: Preliminary Experiments in Levallois Flake Production , 1997 .

[24]  Asif A Ghazanfar,et al.  Response to Lieberman on “Monkey vocal tracts are speech-ready” , 2017, Science Advances.

[25]  P. Lieberman Language Did Not Spring Forth 100,000 Years Ago , 2015, PLoS biology.

[26]  H. Roche,et al.  Early hominid stone tool production and technical skill 2.34 Myr ago in West Turkana, Kenya , 1999, Nature.

[27]  P. Greenfield,et al.  Language, tools and brain: The ontogeny and phylogeny of hierarchically organized sequential behavior , 1991, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[28]  James Steele,et al.  From action to language: comparative perspectives on primate tool use, gesture and the evolution of human language , 2012, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[29]  F. Nottebohm,et al.  Primate Communication and the Gestural Origin of Language [and Comments and Reply] , 1973, Current Anthropology.

[30]  M. Kawato,et al.  Shared neural correlates for language and tool use in Broca's area , 2009, Neuroreport.

[31]  P. Wiessner Embers of society: Firelight talk among the Ju/’hoansi Bushmen , 2014, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[32]  D. McNeill Hand and Mind: What Gestures Reveal about Thought , 1992 .