Effects of task constraint on action dynamics

The actualization of action possibilities (i.e., affordances) can often be accomplished in numerous ways. For instance, an individual could walk over to a rubbish bin to drop an item in or throw the piece of rubbish into the bin from some distance away. The aim of the current study was to investigate the action dynamics that emerge from such under-constrained task or action spaces using an object transportation task. Participants were instructed to transport balls between a starting location and a large wooden box located 9 meters away. The temporal interval between the sequential presentation of balls was manipulated as a control parameter and was expected to influence the distance participants moved prior to throwing or dropping the ball into the target box. A two-parameter state space derived from the Cusp Catastrophe Model was employed to illustrate how behavioral variability emerged as a consequence of the under-constrained task context. Two follow-up experiments demonstrated direct correspondence between model predictions and observed action dynamics as a function of increasing task constraints. Implications for modelling, the theory of affordances, and empirical studies more generally are discussed.

[1]  William Bechtel,et al.  Dynamical Models: An Alternative or Complement to Mechanistic Explanations? , 2011, Top. Cogn. Sci..

[2]  A. Chemero Radical Embodied Cognitive Science , 2009 .

[3]  Ian H. Witten,et al.  Data mining: practical machine learning tools and techniques, 3rd Edition , 1999 .

[4]  G J Savelsbergh,et al.  A developmental transition in prehension modeled as a cusp catastrophe. , 1998, Developmental psychobiology.

[5]  K. Newell,et al.  The scaling of human grip configurations. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[6]  Rick Dale,et al.  Handbook of Research Methods in Social and Personality Psychology: Complex Dynamical Systems in Social and Personality Psychology , 2014 .

[7]  Michael T. Turvey,et al.  Negative hysteresis in the behavioral dynamics of the affordance “graspable” , 2013, Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics.

[8]  Brett R. Fajen,et al.  Affordance-Based Control of Visually Guided Action , 2007 .

[9]  Claudia Carello,et al.  Haptic and Visual Perception of an Affordance for Upright Posture , 1994 .

[10]  Stephen J. Guastello,et al.  Chaos, Catastrophe, and Human Affairs: Applications of Nonlinear Dynamics To Work, Organizations, and Social Evolution , 1995 .

[11]  M. Turvey,et al.  Visually perceiving what is reachable. , 1989 .

[12]  L. S. Mark,et al.  Eyeheight-scaled information about affordances: a study of sitting and stair climbing. , 1987, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[13]  N. Cartwright How the laws of physics lie , 1984 .

[14]  W H Warren,et al.  Perceiving affordances: visual guidance of stair climbing. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[15]  Michael J. Richardson,et al.  Judging and actualizing intrapersonal and interpersonal affordances. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[16]  B. Hopkins,et al.  Developmental transitions: So what's new? , 1998 .

[17]  J. Kelso Phase transitions and critical behavior in human bimanual coordination. , 1984, The American journal of physiology.

[18]  G J Savelsbergh,et al.  Body-scaled ratio as a control parameter for prehension in 5- to 9-year-old children. , 1998, Developmental psychobiology.

[19]  N. Cartwright The Dappled World , 1999 .

[20]  W. Warren,et al.  Visual guidance of walking through apertures: body-scaled information for affordances. , 1987, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[21]  Stephen J. Guastello,et al.  Catastrophe Models for Cognitive Workload and Fatigue in a Vigilance Dual Task , 2014, Hum. Factors.

[22]  Brenda R. J. Jansen,et al.  What response times tell of children's behavior on the balance scale task. , 2003, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[23]  Michael T. Turvey,et al.  Philosophy for the Rest of Cognitive Science , 2011, Top. Cogn. Sci..

[24]  Hermann Haken,et al.  Synergetics: An Introduction , 1983 .

[25]  Lew Hardy,et al.  Testing the Predictions of the Cusp Catastrophe Model of Anxiety and Performance , 1996 .

[26]  René Thom,et al.  Structural stability and morphogenesis , 1977, Pattern Recognit..

[27]  Robert Gilmore,et al.  Catastrophe Theory for Scientists and Engineers , 1981 .

[28]  William Bechtel,et al.  Discovering Complexity: Decomposition and Localization as Strategies in Scientific Research , 2010 .

[29]  Michael J. Richardson,et al.  Complex dynamical systems in social and personality psychology: theory, modeling, and analysis , 2014 .

[30]  T. Järvilehto,et al.  The theory of the organism-environment system: I. Description of the theory , 1998, Integrative physiological and behavioral science : the official journal of the Pavlovian Society.

[31]  A. Tesser,et al.  When Individual Dispositions and Social Pressure Conflict: A Catastrophe , 1980 .

[32]  J. Gibson The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception , 1979 .

[33]  Keith Davids,et al.  Affordance-controlled bifurcations of action patterns in martial arts. , 2006, Nonlinear dynamics, psychology, and life sciences.

[34]  A. Opstal Dynamic Patterns: The Self-Organization of Brain and Behavior , 1995 .