Development and neurophysiology of mentalizing.

The mentalizing (theory of mind) system of the brain is probably in operation from ca. 18 months of age, allowing implicit attribution of intentions and other mental states. Between the ages of 4 and 6 years explicit mentalizing becomes possible, and from this age children are able to explain the misleading reasons that have given rise to a false belief. Neuroimaging studies of mentalizing have so far only been carried out in adults. They reveal a system with three components consistently activated during both implicit and explicit mentalizing tasks: medial prefrontal cortex (MPFC), temporal poles and posterior superior temporal sulcus (STS). The functions of these components can be elucidated, to some extent, from their role in other tasks used in neuroimaging studies. Thus, the MPFC region is probably the basis of the decoupling mechanism that distinguishes mental state representations from physical state representations; the STS region is probably the basis of the detection of agency, and the temporal poles might be involved in access to social knowledge in the form of scripts. The activation of these components in concert appears to be critical to mentalizing.

[1]  N. Modiano The Children. , 1898 .

[2]  Sam T. Pickering A DIRECT DEMONSTRATION , 1909 .

[3]  W. Köhler The Mentality of Apes. , 2018, Nature.

[4]  F. Heider,et al.  An experimental study of apparent behavior , 1944 .

[5]  J. Flavell,et al.  The early development of inferences about the visual percepts of others. , 1974, Child development.

[6]  J. Flavell,et al.  The development in very young children of tacit knowledge concerning visual perception. , 1977, Genetic psychology monographs.

[7]  Roger C. Schank,et al.  Scripts, plans, goals and understanding: an inquiry into human knowledge structures , 1978 .

[8]  Daniel C. Dennett,et al.  Beliefs about beliefs [P&W, SR&B] , 1978, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[9]  D. Premack,et al.  Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? , 1978, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[10]  J. Flavell,et al.  Young children's knowledge about visual perception: Further evidence for the Level 1–Level 2 distinction. , 1981 .

[11]  H. Wimmer,et al.  Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children's understanding of deception , 1983, Cognition.

[12]  H. Wellman,et al.  The acquisition of mental verbs: A systematic investigation of the first reference to mental state , 1983, Cognition.

[13]  B. Bertenthal,et al.  Infant sensitivity to figural coherence in biomechanical motions. , 1984, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[14]  E. Tulving Memory and consciousness. , 1985 .

[15]  H. Wimmer,et al.  “John thinks that Mary thinks that…” attribution of second-order beliefs by 5- to 10-year-old children ☆ , 1985 .

[16]  S. Baron-Cohen,et al.  Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind” ? , 1985, Cognition.

[17]  D. Sperber,et al.  Relevance: Communication and Cognition , 1997 .

[18]  H. Wellman,et al.  Early understanding of mental entities: a reexamination of childhood realism. , 1986, Child development.

[19]  H. Wimmer,et al.  Ignorance versus false belief: a developmental lag in attribution of epistemic states , 1986 .

[20]  A. Leslie Pretense and representation: The origins of "theory of mind." , 1987 .

[21]  E. Balint Memory and consciousness. , 1987, The International journal of psycho-analysis.

[22]  M. Mesulam,et al.  Neural inputs into the temporopolar cortex of the rhesus monkey , 1987, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[23]  H. Wimmer,et al.  Three-year-olds' difficulty with false belief: The case for a conceptual deficit , 1987 .

[24]  P. C. Lee How monkeys see the world: Inside the mind of another species , 1990 .

[25]  P. Harris,et al.  Belief‐Desire Reasoning among Baka Children: Evidence for a Universal Conception of Mind , 1991 .

[26]  A. Gopnik,et al.  Young Children's Ability to Identify the Sources of Their Beliefs. , 1991 .

[27]  G. Butterworth,et al.  What minds have in common is space : Spatial mechanisms serving joint visual attention in infancy , 1991 .

[28]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  A direct demonstration of functional specialization in human visual cortex , 1991, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[29]  Mark H. Johnson,et al.  Biology and Cognitive Development: The Case of Face Recognition , 1993 .

[30]  A. Gopnik,et al.  Natural theories of mind: Evolution, development and simulation of everyday mindreading , 2015 .

[31]  G. Butterworth The ontogeny and phylogeny of joint visual attention. , 1991 .

[32]  P. Harris The work of imagination , 1991 .

[33]  M. Legerstee A review of the animate-inanimate distinction in infancy: Implications for models of social and cognitive knowing , 1992 .

[34]  D. Povinelli,et al.  Young children's (Homo sapiens) understanding of knowledge formation in themselves and others. , 1992, Journal of comparative psychology.

[35]  J. Fodor A theory of the child's theory of mind , 1992, Cognition.

[36]  D. Pandya,et al.  Efferent cortical connections of multimodal cortex of the superior temporal sulcus in the rhesus monkey , 1992, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[37]  A. Syrota,et al.  The Cortical Representation of Speech , 1993, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[38]  M Corbetta Positron emission tomography as a tool to study human vision and attention. , 1993, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[39]  A. Leslie Pretending and believing: issues in the theory of ToMM , 1994, Cognition.

[40]  Helen Tager-Flusberg,et al.  Preschoolers can attribute second-order beliefs , 1994 .

[41]  A. Gopnik,et al.  The theory theory. , 1994 .

[42]  F. Happé An advanced test of theory of mind: Understanding of story characters' thoughts and feelings by able autistic, mentally handicapped, and normal children and adults , 1994, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[43]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  The role of the right hemisphere in the interpretation of figurative aspects of language. A positron emission tomography activation study. , 1994, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[44]  Josef Perner,et al.  Implicit Understanding of Belief , 1994 .

[45]  S. Gelman,et al.  Mapping the Mind: Domain Specificity In Cognition And Culture , 1994 .

[46]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  Other minds in the brain: a functional imaging study of “theory of mind” in story comprehension , 1995, Cognition.

[47]  B. Vogt,et al.  Contributions of anterior cingulate cortex to behaviour. , 1995, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[48]  A. Meltzoff Understanding the Intentions of Others: Re-Enactment of Intended Acts by 18-Month-Old Children. , 1995, Developmental psychology.

[49]  Z. Nadasdy,et al.  Taking the intentional stance at 12 months of age , 1995, Cognition.

[50]  E. Spelke,et al.  Infants' knowledge of object motion and human action. , 1995 .

[51]  D. Povinelli,et al.  Theory of mind: evolutionary history of a cognitive specialization , 1995, Trends in Neurosciences.

[52]  M. Hallett,et al.  Modeling other minds , 1995, Neuroreport.

[53]  P. Strick,et al.  Motor areas of the medial wall: a review of their location and functional activation. , 1996, Cerebral cortex.

[54]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  Functional anatomy of a common semantic system for words and pictures , 1996, Nature.

[55]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  Specific Involvement of Human Parietal Systems and the Amygdala in the Perception of Biological Motion , 1996, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[56]  Daniela K. O'Neill,et al.  Two‐Year‐Old Children's Sensitivity to a Parent's Knowledge State When Making Requests , 1996 .

[57]  G. Fink,et al.  Cerebral Representation of One’s Own Past: Neural Networks Involved in Autobiographical Memory , 1996, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[58]  A. Premack,et al.  Causal cognition : a multidisciplinary debate , 1996 .

[59]  Philip McGuire,et al.  Brain activity during stimulus independent thought. , 1996 .

[60]  Dare A. Baldwin,et al.  The Ontogeny of Social Information Gathering , 1996 .

[61]  V. Tillmann,et al.  Serum leptin through childhood and adolescence , 1997, Clinical endocrinology.

[62]  G. Fink,et al.  Neural activation during selective attention to subjective emotional responses , 1997, Neuroreport.

[63]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Thalamic and temporal cortex input to medial prefrontal cortex in rhesus monkeys , 1997, Experimental Brain Research.

[64]  G. Humphreys,et al.  Segregating Semantic from Phonological Processes during Reading , 1997, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[65]  A. J. Caron,et al.  Infant sensitivity to deviations in dynamic facial-vocal displays: the role of eye regard. , 1997, Developmental psychology.

[66]  R. Lane,et al.  Neural Correlates of Levels of Emotional Awareness: Evidence of an Interaction between Emotion and Attention in the Anterior Cingulate Cortex , 1998, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[67]  J. Perner,et al.  Older (but not younger) siblings facilitate false belief understanding. , 1998, Developmental psychology.

[68]  J. Decety,et al.  Brain Regions Involved in the Perception of Gaze: A PET Study , 1998, NeuroImage.

[69]  T. Allison,et al.  Temporal Cortex Activation in Humans Viewing Eye and Mouth Movements , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[70]  J. Decety,et al.  Top down effect of strategy on the perception of human biological motion: a pet investigation. , 1998, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[71]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Sustained Activity in the Medial Wall during Working Memory Delays , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[72]  David A. Leavens,et al.  Theory of mind in nonhuman primates , 1998, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[73]  Jon Driver,et al.  Adult's Eyes Trigger Shifts of Visual Attention in Human Infants , 1998 .

[74]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Social cognition, joint attention, and communicative competence from 9 to 15 months of age. , 1998, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.

[75]  D. Wolpert,et al.  Central cancellation of self-produced tickle sensation , 1998, Nature Neuroscience.

[76]  B. Repacholi Infants' use of attentional cues to identify the referent of another person's emotional expression. , 1998, Developmental psychology.

[77]  A. Lillard,et al.  Ethnopsychologies: cultural variations in theories of mind. , 1998, Psychological bulletin.

[78]  A. Woodward Infants selectively encode the goal object of an actor's reach , 1998, Cognition.

[79]  C. Frith,et al.  Interacting minds--a biological basis. , 1999, Science.

[80]  J. Haxby,et al.  Attribute-based neural substrates in temporal cortex for perceiving and knowing about objects , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[81]  E. Maguire,et al.  The functional neuroanatomy of comprehension and memory: the importance of prior knowledge. , 1999, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[82]  E. Maguire,et al.  Differential modulation of a common memory retrieval network revealed by positron emission tomography , 1999, Hippocampus.

[83]  J. Allman,et al.  A neuronal morphologic type unique to humans and great apes. , 1999, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[84]  T. Paus,et al.  Cerebral Mechanisms of Hypnotic Induction and Suggestion , 1999, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[85]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Conflict monitoring versus selection-for-action in anterior cingulate cortex , 1999, Nature.

[86]  Animacy and propulsion in infancy: Tracking, waving and reaching to self‐propelled and induced moving objects , 1999 .

[87]  M. Forrester Agency: Its role in mental development , 1999 .

[88]  E. Maguire,et al.  Patterns of hippocampal‐cortical interaction dissociate temporal lobe memory subsystems , 2000, Hippocampus.

[89]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Voluntary orienting is dissociated from target detection in human posterior parietal cortex , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[90]  C. Frith,et al.  Movement and Mind: A Functional Imaging Study of Perception and Interpretation of Complex Intentional Movement Patterns , 2000, NeuroImage.

[91]  J. Downar,et al.  A multimodal cortical network for the detection of changes in the sensory environment , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[92]  H. Critchley,et al.  Cerebral correlates of autonomic cardiovascular arousal: a functional neuroimaging investigation in humans , 2000, The Journal of physiology.

[93]  Richard D. Lane,et al.  Neural correlates of conscious emotional experience. , 2000 .

[94]  H. Wellman,et al.  Young children's understanding of perception, desire, and emotion. , 2000, Child development.

[95]  J. Decety,et al.  A PET Investigation of the Attribution of Intentions with a Nonverbal Task , 2000, NeuroImage.

[96]  R. Blake,et al.  Brain Areas Involved in Perception of Biological Motion , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[97]  J. Haxby,et al.  Distinct representations of eye gaze and identity in the distributed human neural system for face perception , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[98]  T. Allison,et al.  Social perception from visual cues: role of the STS region , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[99]  C. Frith,et al.  Reading the mind in cartoons and stories: an fMRI study of ‘theory of mind’ in verbal and nonverbal tasks , 2000, Neuropsychologia.

[100]  T. Schormann,et al.  Functional delineation of the human occipito-temporal areas related to face and scene processing. A PET study. , 2000, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[101]  R. Campbell,et al.  Evidence from functional magnetic resonance imaging of crossmodal binding in the human heteromodal cortex , 2000, Current Biology.

[102]  T. Braver,et al.  Anterior Cingulate Cortex and Response Conflict : Effects of Response Modality and Processing Domain , 2022 .

[103]  M. Posner,et al.  Cognitive and emotional influences in anterior cingulate cortex , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[104]  Adrian M. Owen,et al.  Dissociative methods in the study of frontal lobe function , 2000 .

[105]  M N Rossor,et al.  Patterns of temporal lobe atrophy in semantic dementia and Alzheimer's disease , 2001, Annals of neurology.

[106]  C. Frith,et al.  Imaging the intentional stance , 2001, NeuroImage.

[107]  B. Bertenthal,et al.  Does Perception of Biological Motion Rely on Specific Brain Regions? , 2001, NeuroImage.

[108]  R. Henson,et al.  Neural activity associated with episodic memory for emotional context , 2001, Neuropsychologia.

[109]  A. Nakamura,et al.  Neural substrates for recognition of familiar voices: a PET study , 2001, Neuropsychologia.

[110]  H. Critchley,et al.  Neuroanatomical basis for first- and second-order representations of bodily states , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[111]  J. J. Guajardo,et al.  How infants make sense of intentional action , 2001 .

[112]  R. Blair,et al.  Dissociation between 'theory of mind' and executive functions in a patient with early left amygdala damage. , 2001, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[113]  Dare A. Baldwin,et al.  Intentions and Intentionality: Foundations of Social Cognition , 2001 .

[114]  C. Polkey,et al.  "Theory of mind" impairments and their relationship to executive functioning following frontal lobe excisions. , 2001, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[115]  E. Funnell Evidence for scripts in semantic dementia: Implications for theories of semantic memory , 2001, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[116]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  An fMRI Investigation of Emotional Engagement in Moral Judgment , 2001, Science.

[117]  Daniel Houser,et al.  A functional imaging study of cooperation in two-person reciprocal exchange , 2001, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[118]  K. Zilles,et al.  Mind Reading: Neural Mechanisms of Theory of Mind and Self-Perspective , 2001, NeuroImage.

[119]  M. Siegal,et al.  Severe Impairment in Grammar Does Not Preclude Theory of Mind , 2001, Neurocase.

[120]  T. Braver,et al.  Anterior cingulate cortex and response conflict: effects of response modality and processing domain. , 2001, Cerebral Cortex.

[121]  M. Mishkin,et al.  The effects of bilateral hippocampal damage on fMRI regional activations and interactions during memory retrieval. , 2001, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[122]  J. Perner,et al.  Actions really do speak louder than words—but only implicitly: Young children's understanding of false belief in action , 2001 .

[123]  G. Shulman,et al.  Medial prefrontal cortex and self-referential mental activity: Relation to a default mode of brain function , 2001, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[124]  P. McGuire,et al.  Cortical substrates for the perception of face actions: an fMRI study of the specificity of activation for seen speech and for meaningless lower-face acts (gurning). , 2001, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[125]  J. Allman,et al.  The Anterior Cingulate Cortex , 2001, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[126]  N. Yeung,et al.  Anterior Cingulate Cortex , 2002 .

[127]  R. Dolan,et al.  An fMRI study of intentional and unintentional (embarrassing) violations of social norms. , 2002, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[128]  Andy C. H. Lee,et al.  “Pray or Prey?” Dissociation of Semantic Memory Retrieval from Episodic Memory Processes Using Positron Emission Tomography and a Novel Homophone Task , 2002, NeuroImage.

[129]  Martin Ingvar,et al.  Imaging cognitive modulation of pain processing , 2002, Pain.

[130]  K. Luan Phan,et al.  Functional Neuroanatomy of Emotion: A Meta-Analysis of Emotion Activation Studies in PET and fMRI , 2002, NeuroImage.

[131]  C. Price,et al.  Retrieval of Visual, Auditory, and Abstract Semantics , 2002, NeuroImage.

[132]  C. Price,et al.  A PET Study of Stimulus- and Task-Induced Semantic Processing , 2002, NeuroImage.

[133]  Evelyn C. Ferstl,et al.  What Does the Frontomedian Cortex Contribute to Language Processing: Coherence or Theory of Mind? , 2002, NeuroImage.

[134]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Motor and cognitive functions of the ventral premotor cortex , 2002, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[135]  Evelyn C. Ferstl,et al.  The Anterior Frontomedian Cortex and Evaluative Judgment: An fMRI Study , 2002, NeuroImage.

[136]  A. Nobre,et al.  The Response of Left Temporal Cortex to Sentences , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[137]  D. Povinelli,et al.  The Mentality of Apes Revisited , 2002 .

[138]  Christopher D. Frith,et al.  Imaging the Intentional Stance in a Competitive Game , 2002, NeuroImage.

[139]  H. Wellman,et al.  Infants' ability to connect gaze and emotional expression to intentional action , 2002, Cognition.

[140]  Aina Puce,et al.  Electrophysiology and brain imaging of biological motion. , 2003, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[141]  Susan C. Johnson Detecting agents. , 2003, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[142]  C. Frith,et al.  “Hey John”: Signals Conveying Communicative Intention toward the Self Activate Brain Regions Associated with “Mentalizing,” Regardless of Modality , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[143]  R. Passingham,et al.  Sleep-Related Consolidation of a Visuomotor Skill: Brain Mechanisms as Assessed by Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[144]  G. Csibra Teleological and referential understanding of action in infancy. , 2003, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[145]  P. Skudlarski,et al.  The role of the fusiform face area in social cognition: implications for the pathobiology of autism. , 2003, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[146]  IMPLICATIONS FOR THEORIES OF SEMANTIC MEMORY , 2004 .

[147]  B. Sodian,et al.  Infants' Understanding of Looking, Pointing, and Reaching as Cues to Goal-Directed Action , 2004 .

[148]  R. Byrne,et al.  Machiavellian intelligence , 2022 .