Do alternative names block young and older adults’ retrieval of proper names?

This study evaluates whether tip of the tongue experiences (TOTs) are caused by a more accessible word which blocks retrieval of the target word, especially for older adults. In a "competitor priming" paradigm, young and older adults produced the name of a famous character (e.g., Eliza Doolittle) in response to a question and subsequently named a picture of a famous actor or actress depicting this character (e.g., Audrey Hepburn as Eliza Doolittle). Older adults produced more TOTs than young adults, but prior production of a related character name did not affect TOTs, although it did reduce incorrect responses. There were no age differences in knowledge of films and TV and thus the age-related increase in TOTs is not because older adults have more relevant knowledge. The findings are compatible with models in which alternate words are a consequence not a cause of TOT.

[1]  K Bock,et al.  The tip-of-the-tongue phenomenon: Blocking or partial activation? , 1992, Memory & cognition.

[2]  D. Gopher,et al.  Attention and performance XVII: Cognitive regulation of performance: Interaction of theory and application. , 1999 .

[3]  T. Carr,et al.  Inhibitory Processes in Attention, Memory and Language , 1994 .

[4]  Gregory V. Jones,et al.  Phonological blocking in the tip of the tongue state , 1987, Cognition.

[5]  G. Cohen,et al.  Memory for proper names: age differences in retrieval , 1986 .

[6]  Cynthia P. May,et al.  Inhibitory control, circadian arousal, and age. , 1999 .

[7]  A. Damasio,et al.  Brain and language. , 1993, Scientific American.

[8]  L L Jacoby,et al.  Separating habit and recollection in young and older adults: effects of elaborative processing and distinctiveness. , 1999, Psychology and aging.

[9]  Thomas M. Hess,et al.  Aging and cognition : knowledge organization and utilization , 1990 .

[10]  Eugene A. Lovelace,et al.  Healthy older adults’ perceptions of their memory functioning and use of mnemonics , 1990 .

[11]  B. Ober,et al.  Naturally occurring and experimentally induced tip-of-the-tongue experiences in three adult age groups. , 1999, Psychology and aging.

[12]  N. L. Bowles,et al.  Age and rate of activation in semantic memory. , 1994, Psychology and aging.

[13]  D. G. MacKay,et al.  Memory, language, and ageing. , 1997, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[14]  Lise Abrams,et al.  Does priming specific syllables during tip-of-the-tongue states facilitate word retrieval in older adults? , 2002 .

[15]  Gregory V. Jones,et al.  Back to Woodworth: Role of interlopers in the tip-of-the-tongue phenomenon , 1989, Memory & cognition.

[16]  Michael C. Anderson,et al.  Mechanisms of inhibition in long-term memory: A new taxonomy. , 1994 .

[17]  Larry L. Jacoby,et al.  DECEIVING THE ELDERLY: EFFECTS OF ACCESSIBILITY BIAS IN CUED-RECALL PERFORMANCE , 1999 .

[18]  Muriel Evrard,et al.  Ageing and Lexical Access to Common and Proper Names in Picture Naming , 2002, Brain and Language.

[19]  L. Hasher,et al.  Directed ignoring: Inhibitory regulation of working memory. , 1994 .

[20]  G. Cohen,et al.  Memory for proper names: a review. , 1993, Memory.

[21]  D. Burke,et al.  Phonological priming effects on word retrieval and tip-of-the-tongue experiences in young and older adults. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[22]  L. Henry,et al.  Everyday Memory, Actions, and Absent-Mindedness , 1986 .

[23]  A. S. Brown,et al.  Age-related changes in the tip-of-the-tongue experience. , 1996, The American journal of psychology.

[24]  S. Monsell,et al.  Inhibition of Spoken Word Production by Priming a Semantic Competitor , 1994 .

[25]  Alan S. Brown,et al.  A review of the tip-of-the-tongue experience. , 1991, Psychological bulletin.

[26]  Tim Valentine,et al.  The cognitive psychology of proper names: On the importance of being Ernest , 1996 .

[27]  Trevor A. Harley,et al.  What causes a tip-of-the-tongue state? Evidence for lexical neighbourhood effects in speech production , 1998 .

[28]  Lynn Hasher,et al.  Working Memory, Comprehension, and Aging: A Review and a New View , 1988 .

[29]  D. Dahlgren,et al.  Impact of knowledge and age on tip-of-the-tongue rates. , 1998, Experimental aging research.

[30]  D. Schacter The seven sins of memory. Insights from psychology and cognitive neuroscience. , 1999, The American psychologist.

[31]  J. Hanley,et al.  The tip-of-the-tongue phenomenon: Do experimenter-presented interlopers have any effect? , 1992, Cognition.

[32]  A Wingfield,et al.  Response Latencies in Naming Objects , 1965, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[33]  Kathleen Rastle,et al.  Priming the Tip of the Tongue: Effects of Prior Processing on Word Retrieval in Young and Older Adults , 1996 .

[34]  Donald G. MacKay,et al.  On the tip of the tongue: What causes word finding failures in young and older adults? , 1991 .

[35]  E. Maylor,et al.  Recognizing and naming faces: aging, memory retrieval, and the tip of the tongue state. , 1990, Journal of gerontology.

[36]  D. G. MacKay,et al.  Chapter Five Cognition and Aging: A Theory of New Learning and the Use of Old Connections , 1990 .