The impact of generic language about ability on children's achievement motivation.

Nuances in how adults talk about ability may have important consequences for children's sustained involvement and success in an activity. In this study, I tested the hypothesis that children would be less motivated while performing a novel activity if they were told that boys or girls in general are good at this activity (generic language) than if they were told that a particular boy or girl is good at it (non-generic language). Generic language may be detrimental because it expresses normative societal expectations regarding performance. If these expectations are negative, they may cause children to worry about confirming them; if positive, they may cause worries about failing to meet them. Moreover, generic statements may be threatening because they imply that performance is the result of stable traits rather than effort. Ninety-seven 4- to 7-year-olds were asked to play a game in which they succeeded at first but then made a few mistakes. Since young children remain optimistic in achievement situations until the possibility of failure is made clear, I hypothesized that 4- and 5-year-olds would not be affected by the implications of generic language until after they made mistakes; 6- and 7-year-olds, however, may be susceptible earlier. As expected, the older children who heard that boys or girls are good at this game displayed lower motivation (e.g., more negative emotions, lower perceived competence) from the start, while they were still succeeding and receiving praise. Four- and 5-year-olds who heard these generic statements had a similar reaction, but only after they made mistakes. These findings demonstrate that exposure to generic language about ability can be an obstacle to children's motivation and, potentially, their success.

[1]  S. Gelman,et al.  Generic noun phrases in mother–child conversations , 1998, Journal of Child Language.

[2]  Alice ter Meulen,et al.  Genericity: An Introduction , 1995 .

[3]  Sandra R. Waxman,et al.  Weaving a Lexicon , 2004 .

[4]  D. Stapel,et al.  Retracted:It's all in the timing: measuring emotional reactions to stereotype threat before and after taking a test , 2006 .

[5]  C. Dweck Mindset: The New Psychology of Success , 2006 .

[6]  K. Fujita,et al.  Stereotype performance boosts: the impact of self-relevance and the manner of stereotype activation. , 2002, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[7]  Francis Jeffry Pelletier,et al.  The Generic book , 1997 .

[8]  Margaret Shih,et al.  Stereotype Susceptibility in Children: Effects of Identity Activation on Quantitative Performance , 2001, Psychological science.

[9]  E. Markman,et al.  The generic/nongeneric distinction influences how children interpret new information about social others. , 2011, Child development.

[10]  C. Dweck,et al.  Person versus process praise and criticism: implications for contingent self-worth and coping. , 1999 .

[11]  S. Gelman,et al.  Preschool children use linguistic form class and pragmatic cues to interpret generics. , 2003, Child development.

[12]  T. Roberts,et al.  Gender and the influence of evaluations on self-assessments in achievement settings. , 1991, Psychological bulletin.

[13]  Jennifer K. Bosson,et al.  When saying and doing diverge: The effects ofstereotype threat on self-reported versus nonverbal anxiety , 2004 .

[14]  Twila Tardif,et al.  A cross-linguistic comparison of generic noun phrases in English and Mandarin , 1998, Cognition.

[15]  E. Markman,et al.  Subtle Linguistic Cues Affect Children's Motivation , 2007, Psychological science.

[16]  Sandra R Waxman,et al.  Mother-child conversations about pictures and objects: referring to categories and individuals. , 2005, Child development.

[17]  D. Stipek Children's perceptions of their own and their classmates' ability. , 1981 .

[18]  N. Haslam,et al.  Psychological essentialism and stereotype endorsement , 2006 .

[19]  Susan A. Gelman,et al.  Inferring Properties from Categories versus Inferring Categories from Properties: The Case of Gender. , 1986 .

[20]  Barbara W Sarnecka,et al.  Generic Language in Parent-Child Conversations , 2008, Language learning and development : the official journal of the Society for Language Development.

[21]  Andrei Cimpian,et al.  Preschool children’s use of cues to generic meaning , 2008, Cognition.

[22]  Sandeep Prasada,et al.  Principled and statistical connections in common sense conception , 2006, Cognition.

[23]  J. Gross,et al.  Signaling Threat , 2007, Psychological science.

[24]  Dedre Gentner,et al.  Effects of Language and Similarity on Comparison Processing , 2009, Language learning and development : the official journal of the Society for Language Development.

[25]  C. Dweck,et al.  The impact of children's static versus dynamic conceptions of people on stereotype formation , 1999 .

[26]  Carrie B. Fried,et al.  Reducing the Effects of Stereotype Threat on African American College Students by Shaping Theories of Intelligence , 2002 .

[27]  Marianne G. Taylor,et al.  Mother-child conversations about gender : understanding the acquisition of essentialist beliefs , 2004 .

[28]  Jon Star,et al.  Children's interpretation of generic noun phrases. , 2002, Developmental psychology.

[29]  Carol S. Dweck,et al.  Motivational processes affecting learning. , 1986 .

[30]  Sapna Cheryan,et al.  When Positive Stereotypes Threaten Intellectual Performance: The Psychological Hazards of “Model Minority” Status , 2000, Psychological science.

[31]  D. Watson,et al.  Constructing validity: Basic issues in objective scale development , 1995 .

[32]  Gail D. Heyman,et al.  Young children's vulnerability to self-blame and helplessness: relationship to beliefs about goodness. , 1992, Child development.

[33]  Susan A. Gelman,et al.  Learning Words for Kinds: Generic Noun Phrases in Acquisition. , 2004 .

[34]  C. Chambers,et al.  When hearsay trumps evidence: How generic language guides preschoolers’ inferences about unfamiliar things , 2008 .

[35]  Andrei Cimpian,et al.  Information learned from generic language becomes central to children’s biological concepts: Evidence from their open-ended explanations , 2009, Cognition.

[36]  D. Schunk Ability Versus Effort Attributional Feedback: Differential Effects on Self-Efficacy and Achievement , 1983 .

[37]  C. Dweck Self-Theories: Their Role in Motivation, Personality, and Development. Essays in Social Psychology. , 1999 .

[38]  Daniel Brickman,et al.  Preschoolers' Responses to Social Comparisons Involving Relative Failure , 2008, Psychological science.

[39]  Carol S. Dweck,et al.  Determinants of academic achievement: The interaction of children's achievement orientations with skill area. , 1984 .

[40]  Sandeep Prasada,et al.  Acquiring generic knowledge , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[41]  R. Josephs,et al.  A burden of proof: Stereotype relevance and gender differences in math performance. , 1999 .

[42]  R. Endsley,et al.  The effects of sex-Typed labeling on preschool children's information-Seeking and retention , 1983 .

[43]  S. Gelman,et al.  Beyond labeling: the role of maternal input in the acquisition of richly structured categories. , 1991, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.

[44]  A. Papafragou On generics * , 1996 .

[45]  S. Gelman,et al.  The Essential Child : Origins of Essentialism in Everyday Thought , 2003 .

[46]  John H. Flavell,et al.  Developmental changes in memorization processes , 1970 .

[47]  John G. Nicholls,et al.  The Development of the Concepts of Effort and Ability, Perception of Academic Attainment, and the Understanding that Difficult Tasks Require More Ability. , 1978 .

[48]  C. Martin,et al.  Children's gender-based reasoning about toys. , 1995, Child development.

[49]  C. Dweck,et al.  Helplessness in early childhood: the role of contingent worth. , 1995, Child development.

[50]  C. Dweck,et al.  Praise for intelligence can undermine children's motivation and performance. , 1998, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[51]  S. Spencer,et al.  Consuming Images: How Television Commercials that Elicit Stereotype Threat Can Restrain Women Academically and Professionally , 2002 .

[52]  Michelle A. Hollander,et al.  Generic language and judgements about category membership: Can generics highlight properties as central? , 2009, Language and cognitive processes.

[53]  C. Dweck Self-theories and goals: their role in motivation, personality, and development. , 1990, Nebraska Symposium on Motivation. Nebraska Symposium on Motivation.

[54]  C. Dweck,et al.  The development of trait explanations and self-evaluations in the academic and social domains. , 1986, Child development.

[55]  R. Peterson A Meta-analysis of Cronbach's Coefficient Alpha , 1994 .

[56]  C. Steele,et al.  Stereotype threat and the intellectual test performance of African Americans. , 1995, Journal of personality and social psychology.