Perception and action influences on discrete and reciprocal bimanual coordination

For nearly four decades bimanual coordination, “a prototype of complex motor skills” and apparent “window into the design of the brain,” has been intensively studied. Past research has focused on describing and modeling the constraints that allow the production of some coordination patterns while limiting effective performance of other bimanual coordination patterns. More recently researchers have identified a coalition of perception-action constraints that hinder the effective production of bimanual skills. The result has been that given specially designed contexts where one or more of these constraints are minimized, bimanual skills once thought difficult, if not impossible, to effectively produce without very extensive practice can be executed effectively with little or no practice. The challenge is to understand how these contextual constraints interact to allow or inhibit the production of complex bimanual coordination skills. In addition, the factors affecting the stability of bimanual coordination tasks needs to be re-conceptualized in terms of perception-related constraints arising from the environmental context in which performance is conducted and action constraints resident in the neuromotor system.

[1]  Eric L. Amazeen,et al.  Visual–spatial and anatomical constraints interact in a bimanual coordination task with transformed visual feedback , 2008, Experimental Brain Research.

[2]  K. Zilles,et al.  The role of ventral medial wall motor areas in bimanual co-ordination. A combined lesion and activation study. , 1999, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[3]  A. Fuchs,et al.  Coordination: Neural, Behavioral and Social Dynamics , 2008 .

[4]  S. Swinnen,et al.  Interaction of directional, neuromuscular and egocentric constraints on the stability of preferred bimanual coordination patterns. , 2003, Human movement science.

[5]  Charles H. Shea,et al.  The role of auditory and visual models in the production of bimanual tapping patterns , 2012, Experimental Brain Research.

[6]  J A Kelso,et al.  Dynamic pattern generation in behavioral and neural systems. , 1988, Science.

[7]  J. Kelso,et al.  Environmentally-specified patterns of movement coordination in normal and split-brain subjects , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[8]  R. Carson Manual asymmetries: in defense of a multifactorial account. , 1989, Journal of motor behavior.

[9]  Paul Van Hecke,et al.  Brain Areas Involved in Interlimb Coordination: A Distributed Network , 2001, NeuroImage.

[10]  John J. Buchanan,et al.  Learning a single limb multijoint coordination pattern: the impact of a mechanical constraint on the coordination dynamics of learning and transfer , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[11]  M. Turvey,et al.  Phase transitions and critical fluctuations in the visual coordination of rhythmic movements between people. , 1990 .

[12]  S. P. Swinnen,et al.  Relative Phase Alterations During Bimanual Skill Acquisition. , 1995, Journal of motor behavior.

[13]  Gavin Breslin,et al.  Modelling relative motion to facilitate intra-limb coordination. , 2005, Human movement science.

[14]  J. Buchanan,et al.  Consistently modeling the same movement strategy is more important than model skill level in observational learning contexts. , 2014, Acta psychologica.

[15]  G. Wulf Attentional focus and motor learning: a review of 15 years , 2013 .

[16]  G P Bingham,et al.  Visual perception of the relative phasing of human limb movements , 1999, Perception & psychophysics.

[17]  M. Himmelbach,et al.  fMRI study of bimanual coordination , 2000, Neuropsychologia.

[18]  A. Baddeley Exploring the Central Executive , 1996 .

[19]  Peter J. Beek,et al.  Interlimb coupling strength scales with movement amplitude , 2008, Neuroscience Letters.

[20]  Andreas Daffertshofer,et al.  Multivariate time–frequency analysis of electromagnetic brain activity during bimanual motor learning , 2007, NeuroImage.

[21]  H. Haken,et al.  A stochastic theory of phase transitions in human hand movement , 1986, Biological Cybernetics.

[22]  Peter J. Beek,et al.  Laterally focused attention modulates asymmetric coupling in rhythmic interlimb coordination , 2008, Psychological research.

[23]  P. Cavallari,et al.  Differential control of in-phase and anti-phase coupling of rhythmic movements of ipsilateral hand and foot , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[24]  S. Swinnen Intermanual coordination: From behavioural principles to neural-network interactions , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[25]  J. Kelso,et al.  Intentional switching between patterns of bimanual coordination depends on the intrinsic dynamics of the patterns. , 1990, Journal of motor behavior.

[26]  Dagmar Sternad,et al.  Dynamics of 1:2 Coordination: Sources of Symmetry Breaking. , 1999, Journal of motor behavior.

[27]  John J Buchanan,et al.  One-to-One and Polyrhythmic Temporal Coordination in Bimanual Circle Tracing , 2006, Journal of motor behavior.

[28]  B. Preilowski,et al.  Possible contribution of the anterior forebrain commissures to bilateral motor coordination. , 1972, Neuropsychologia.

[29]  J. Kelso Phase transitions and critical behavior in human bimanual coordination. , 1984, The American journal of physiology.

[30]  Peter J. Beek,et al.  Distinguishing between the effects of frequency and amplitude on interlimb coupling in tapping a 2:3 polyrhythm , 1998, Experimental Brain Research.

[31]  Michael T. Turvey,et al.  Attention and Handedness in Bimanual Coordination Dynamics , 1997 .

[32]  W Spijkers,et al.  Temporal coordination of alternative and simultaneous aiming movements of constrained timing structure , 1994, Psychological research.

[33]  Charles H. Shea,et al.  Bimanual 1:1 with 90° continuous relative phase: difficult or easy! , 2009, Experimental Brain Research.

[34]  Timothy D. Lee,et al.  Part and whole perceptual-motor practice of a polyrhythm , 2003, Neuroscience Letters.

[35]  Gabriele Wulf,et al.  Continuous Concurrent Feedback Degrades Skill Learning: Implications for Training and Simulation , 1997, Hum. Factors.

[36]  S. Swinnen,et al.  Interlimb coordination: Learning and transfer under different feedback conditions , 1997 .

[37]  John J Buchanan,et al.  Specificity in practice benefits learning in novice models and variability in demonstration benefits observational practice , 2010, Psychological research.

[38]  H N Zelaznik,et al.  Spatial Conceptual Influences on the Coordination of Bimanual Actions: When a Dual Task Becomes a Single Task , 2001, Journal of motor behavior.

[39]  David L Wright,et al.  Generalization of action knowledge following observational learning. , 2011, Acta psychologica.

[40]  R. Ivry,et al.  The temporal representation of in-phase and anti-phase movements. , 2007, Human movement science.

[41]  Charles H. Shea,et al.  Reacting while moving: influence of right limb movement on left limb reaction , 2013, Experimental Brain Research.

[42]  Andrew D. Wilson,et al.  Human movement coordination implicates relative direction as the information for relative phase , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.

[43]  J. S. Scott Kelso,et al.  Haptic information stabilizes and destabilizes coordination dynamics , 2001, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[44]  J. Kelso,et al.  Action-Perception as a Pattern Formation Process , 2018, Attention and Performance XIII.

[45]  S P Swinnen,et al.  Interaction of neuromuscular, spatial and visual constraints on hand-foot coordination dynamics. , 2005, Human movement science.

[46]  Charles H. Shea,et al.  A guide to performing difficult bimanual coordination tasks: just follow the yellow brick road , 2013, Experimental Brain Research.

[47]  R. Ivry,et al.  Callosotomy patients exhibit temporal uncoupling during continuous bimanual movements , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.

[48]  J. Kelso,et al.  Evolution of behavioral attractors with learning: nonequilibrium phase transitions. , 1992 .

[49]  Andras Semjen,et al.  Instabilities during antiphase bimanual movements: are ipsilateral pathways involved? , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[50]  Peter J. Beek,et al.  Multifrequency coordination in bimanual tapping: Asymmetrical coupling and signs of supercriticality. , 1995 .

[51]  J. Summers,et al.  The acquisition of bimanual coordination is mediated by anisotropic coupling between the hands. , 2002, Human movement science.

[52]  John J. Buchanan,et al.  Learning an environment–actor coordination skill: visuomotor transformation and coherency of perceptual structure , 2009, Experimental Brain Research.

[53]  Robert A. Bjork,et al.  Retrieval practice and the maintenance of knowledge. , 1988 .

[54]  R. Carson Neural pathways mediating bilateral interactions between the upper limbs , 2005, Brain Research Reviews.

[55]  Geoffrey P. Bingham,et al.  A Perceptually Driven Dynamical Model of Bimanual Rhythmic Movement (and Phase Perception) , 2004 .

[56]  Karl M. Newell,et al.  Visual information gain and task asymmetry interact in bimanual force coordination and control , 2011, Experimental Brain Research.

[57]  Charles H. Shea,et al.  Using scanning trials to assess intrinsic coordination dynamics , 2009, Neuroscience Letters.

[58]  J. A. S. Kelso,et al.  Order in Time: How the Cooperation Between the Hands Informs the Design of the Brain , 1988 .

[59]  Young U. Ryu,et al.  Observational practice of relative but not absolute motion features in a single-limb multi-joint coordination task , 2008, Experimental Brain Research.

[60]  G. Wulf,et al.  The learning advantages of an external focus of attention in golf. , 1999, Research quarterly for exercise and sport.

[61]  P J Beek,et al.  Movement-Related Sensory Feedback Mediates the Learning of a New Bimanual Relative Phase Pattern , 2005, Journal of motor behavior.

[62]  S. Swinnen,et al.  Two hands, one brain: cognitive neuroscience of bimanual skill , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[63]  J. Summers,et al.  The influence of perceptual and motor factors on bimanual coordination in a polyrhythmic tapping task , 1993, Psychological research.

[64]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Goal-selection and movement-related conflict during bimanual reaching movements. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.

[65]  R. Schmidt,et al.  Reduced frequency of knowledge of results enhances motor skill learning. , 1990 .

[66]  M. Kinsbourne The cerebral basis of lateral asymmetries in attention. , 1970, Acta psychologica.

[67]  D Deutsch,et al.  The generation of two isochronous sequences in parallel , 1983, Perception & psychophysics.

[68]  M Allen Models of hemispheric specialization. , 1983, Psychological bulletin.

[69]  W A Sparrow,et al.  Performance on trials without knowledge results (KR) in reduced relative frequency presentations of KR. , 1992, Journal of motor behavior.

[70]  Romeo Chua,et al.  The Role of Video in Facilitating Perception and Action of a Novel Coordination Movement , 2003, Journal of motor behavior.

[71]  G. Wulf,et al.  Extended Book Review: Attention and Motor Skill Learning , 2007 .

[72]  H Carnahan,et al.  On the role of knowledge of results in motor learning: exploring the guidance hypothesis. , 1990, Journal of motor behavior.

[73]  R. Ivry,et al.  A Cognitive Neuroscience Perspective on Bimanual Coordination and Interference , 2004 .

[74]  M. Peters,et al.  Does Handedness Play a Role in the Coordination of Bimanual Movement , 1994 .

[75]  G P Bingham,et al.  Visual perception of mean relative phase and phase variability. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[76]  David E. Sherwood,et al.  Interlimb amplitude differences, spatial assimilations, and the temporal structure of rapid bimanual movements , 1994 .

[77]  Stephan P Swinnen,et al.  AGE-RELATED DEFICITS IN MOTOR LEARNING AND DIFFERENCES IN FEEDBACK PROCESSING DURING THE PRODUCTION OF A BIMANUAL COORDINATION PATTERN. , 1998, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[78]  A. J. Kovacs,et al.  Perceptual influences on Fitts’ law , 2008, Experimental Brain Research.

[79]  E. Vaadia,et al.  Neural interactions between motor cortical hemispheres during bimanual and unimanual arm movements , 2001, The European journal of neuroscience.

[80]  Charles H. Shea,et al.  Impossible is nothing: 5:3 and 4:3 multi-frequency bimanual coordination , 2010, Experimental Brain Research.

[81]  Peter J. Beek,et al.  Frequency-induced phase transitions in bimanual tapping , 1995, Biological Cybernetics.

[82]  M. Turvey,et al.  Symmetry, broken symmetry, and handedness in bimanual coordination dynamics , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[83]  Hermann Haken Synergetik: Nichtgleichgewichte, Phasenübergänge und Selbstorganisation , 2005, Naturwissenschaften.

[84]  J. Summers,et al.  Coordination dynamics and attentional costs of continuous and discontinuous bimanual circle drawing movements. , 2008, Human movement science.

[85]  Paul Van Hecke,et al.  Internal vs external generation of movements: differential neural pathways involved in bimanual coordination performed in the presence or absence of augmented visual feedback , 2003, NeuroImage.

[86]  S. P. Swinnen,et al.  The coalition of constraints during coordination of the ipsilateral and heterolateral limbs , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.

[87]  J. Kelso,et al.  Governing coordination: behavioural principles and neural correlates , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[88]  R. Riener,et al.  Augmented visual, auditory, haptic, and multimodal feedback in motor learning: A review , 2012, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.

[89]  R J Jagacinski,et al.  Can people tap concurrent bimanual rhythms independently? , 1998, Journal of motor behavior.

[90]  G. Wulf,et al.  An External Focus of Attention Enhances Golf Shot Accuracy in Beginners and Experts , 2007, Research quarterly for exercise and sport.

[91]  Charles H. Shea,et al.  Amplitude differences, spatial assimilation, and integrated feedback in bimanual coordination , 2010, Experimental Brain Research.

[92]  H. Haken,et al.  A theoretical model of phase transitions in human hand movements , 2004, Biological Cybernetics.

[93]  John J Buchanan,et al.  The Interaction of Tactile Information and Movement Amplitude in a Multijoint Bimanual Circle-Tracing Task: Phase Transitions and Loss of Stability , 2005, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[94]  Dagmar Sternad,et al.  Dynamics of 1:2 Coordination: Generalizing Relative Phase to n:m Rhythms. , 1999, Journal of motor behavior.

[95]  R. Ivry,et al.  Moving to Directly Cued Locations Abolishes Spatial Interference During Bimanual Actions , 2001, Psychological science.

[96]  Charles H. Shea,et al.  Rhythmical bimanual force production: homologous and non-homologous muscles , 2014, Experimental Brain Research.

[97]  Geoffrey P. Bingham,et al.  Chapter 18 Another timing variable composed of state variables: Phase perception and phase driven oscillators , 2004 .

[98]  Michael T. Turvey,et al.  Skill Acquisition: An Event Approach with Special Reference to Searching for the Optimum of a Function of Several Variables , 1978 .

[99]  Franco Lepore,et al.  Multisensory gain within and across hemispaces in simple and choice reaction time paradigms , 2011, Experimental Brain Research.

[100]  Natalia Dounskaia,et al.  Preferred and induced coordination modes during the acquisition of bimanual movements with a 2 :1 frequency ratio , 1997 .

[101]  R. Schmidt,et al.  New Conceptualizations of Practice: Common Principles in Three Paradigms Suggest New Concepts for Training , 1992 .

[102]  M. Peters,et al.  Coordination of the two hands and effects of attentional manipulation in the production of a bimanual 2:3 polyrhythm , 1989 .

[103]  Daniel Cattaert,et al.  Simulating a neural cross-talk model for between-hand interference during bimanual circle drawing , 1999, Biological Cybernetics.

[104]  M. Turvey,et al.  Handedness and the asymmetric dynamics of bimanual rhythmic coordination. , 1995 .

[105]  Charles H. Shea,et al.  Auditory Model , 2008, Research quarterly for exercise and sport.

[106]  Dana Maslovat,et al.  Observational practice benefits are limited to perceptual improvements in the acquisition of a novel coordination skill , 2010, Experimental Brain Research.

[107]  P. V. Wieringen,et al.  Coupling strength in tapping a 2:3 polyrhythm , 1995 .

[108]  C. Shea,et al.  Increasing the distance of an external focus of attention enhances learning , 2003, Psychological research.

[109]  Charles H. Shea,et al.  Effects of an Auditory Model on the Learning of Relative and Absolute Timing , 2001, Journal of motor behavior.

[110]  W. Prinz,et al.  Perceptual basis of bimanual coordination , 2001, Nature.

[111]  Pier-Giorgio Zanone,et al.  Attentional load associated with performing and stabilizing preferred bimanual patterns , 1999 .

[112]  S. Swinnen,et al.  Isofrequency and Multifrequency: Coordination Patterns as a Function of the Planes of Motion , 1997 .

[113]  Andreas Daffertshofer,et al.  A model for phase transitions in human hand movements during multifrequency tapping , 1996 .

[114]  Chaoyi Wang,et al.  Overcoming the guidance effect in motor skill learning: feedback all the time can be beneficial , 2012, Experimental Brain Research.

[115]  Jeff Miller,et al.  Divided attention: Evidence for coactivation with redundant signals , 1982, Cognitive Psychology.

[116]  Bruce Abernethy,et al.  The contribution of inherent and incidental constraints to intentional switching between patterns of bimanual coordination , 1996 .

[117]  G Aschersleben,et al.  Synchronizing actions with events: The role of sensory information , 1995, Perception & psychophysics.

[118]  Justine Sergent,et al.  Music, the brain and Ravel , 1993, Trends in Neurosciences.

[119]  S. P. Swinnen,et al.  Dynamics of learning and transfer of muscular and spatial relative phase in bimanual coordination: evidence for abstract directional codes , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[120]  Richard A. Schmidt,et al.  Frequent Augmented Feedback Can Degrade Learning: Evidence and Interpretations , 1991 .

[121]  C. Wright,et al.  Temporal rescaling of simple and complex ratios in rhythmic tapping. , 1995 .

[122]  J. Kelso,et al.  Nonequilibrium phase transitions in coordinated biological motion: critical fluctuations , 1986 .

[123]  Herbert Heuer,et al.  Intermanual Interactions Related to Movement Amplitudes and Endpoint Locations , 2006, Journal of motor behavior.

[124]  A. Opstal Dynamic Patterns: The Self-Organization of Brain and Behavior , 1995 .

[125]  Franz Mechsner,et al.  Do muscles matter for coordinated action? , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[126]  D Goodman,et al.  On the nature of human interlimb coordination. , 1979, Science.

[127]  W. THOMSON A Constant form of Daniell's Battery , 1871, Nature.

[128]  Yong Li,et al.  Proprioceptive regulation of interlimb behavior: interference between passive movement and active coordination dynamics , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.

[129]  Stefan Panzer,et al.  Increasingly complex bimanual multi-frequency coordination patterns are equally easy to perform with on-line relative velocity feedback , 2011, Experimental Brain Research.

[130]  Peter J. Beek,et al.  Dynamical Models of Movement Coordination , 1995 .

[131]  D Goodman,et al.  On the coordination of two-handed movements. , 1979, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[132]  Charles H Shea,et al.  Auditory Model Enhances Relative-Timing Learning , 2002, Journal of motor behavior.

[133]  J. Kelso,et al.  A quantitative approach to understanding the formation and change of coordinated movement patterns. , 1989, Journal of motor behavior.

[134]  C. MacKenzie,et al.  Bimanual Movement Control: Information processing and Interaction Effects , 1984 .

[135]  Sarah R Hurley,et al.  The influence of augmented feedback and prior learning on the acquisition of a new bimanual coordination pattern. , 2006, Human movement science.

[136]  M. T. Elliott,et al.  Multisensory cues improve sensorimotor synchronisation , 2010, The European journal of neuroscience.

[137]  M. Peters Do feedback processing, output variability, and spatial complexity account for manual asymmetries? , 1989, Journal of motor behavior.

[138]  Yannick Blandin,et al.  Specificity of practice: interaction between concurrent sensory information and terminal feedback. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[139]  Visual Multifrequency Entrainment: Can 1:2, 2:3, and 3:4 Coordination Occur Spontaneously? , 2014, Journal of motor behavior.

[140]  Pier-Giorgio Zanone,et al.  Coordination dynamics of learning and transfer across different effector systems. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[141]  J. Buchanan,et al.  Amplitude Scaling in a Bimanual Circle-Drawing Task: Pattern Switching and End-Effector Variability , 2004, Journal of motor behavior.

[142]  Andrew D. Wilson,et al.  Learning a coordinated rhythmic movement with task-appropriate coordination feedback , 2010, Experimental Brain Research.

[143]  J. Adams,et al.  A closed-loop theory of motor learning. , 1971, Journal of motor behavior.

[144]  R. Schmidt,et al.  Knowledge of results and motor learning: a review and critical reappraisal. , 1984, Psychological bulletin.

[145]  Michael T. Turvey,et al.  Resonance Constraints on Rhythmic Movement , 1993 .

[146]  Charles H Shea,et al.  Perceptual and attentional influences on continuous 2:1 and 3:2 multi-frequency bimanual coordination. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[147]  David R. Collins,et al.  Perceptual coupling in rhythmic movement coordination: stable perception leads to stable action , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.

[148]  J. Kelso,et al.  Social coordination dynamics: Measuring human bonding , 2008, Social neuroscience.

[149]  Charles H. Shea,et al.  Bimanual Fitts’ tasks: Kelso, Southard, and Goodman, 1979 revisited , 2011, Experimental Brain Research.

[150]  Daniel Cattaert,et al.  Bimanual coordination during rhythmic movements in the absence of somatosensory feedback. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[151]  S. A. Wallace,et al.  Order parameters for the neural organization of single, multijoint limb movement patterns , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[152]  Peter J Beek,et al.  Unraveling interlimb interactions underlying bimanual coordination. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[153]  Charles H. Shea,et al.  The learning of 90° continuous relative phase with and without Lissajous feedback: external and internally generated bimanual coordination. , 2011, Acta psychologica.

[154]  Nicole Wenderoth,et al.  Changes in Brain Activation during the Acquisition of a Multifrequency Bimanual Coordination Task: From the Cognitive Stage to Advanced Levels of Automaticity , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[155]  Mitsuo Kawato,et al.  Two coupled oscillators as a model for the coordinated finger tapping by both hands , 1980, Biological Cybernetics.

[156]  J. Summers,et al.  Attentional costs of coordinating homologous and non-homologous limbs. , 2004, Human movement science.

[157]  Jörn Diedrichsen,et al.  Bimanual interference associated with the selection of target locations. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[158]  Carol A. Putnam,et al.  On the Space-Time Structure of Human Interlimb Co-Ordination , 1983, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[159]  I. Bilodeau,et al.  Variable frequency of knowledge of results and the learning of a simple skill. , 1958, Journal of experimental psychology.

[160]  S. Bogacz Understanding How Speed Affects Performance of Polyrhythms: Transferring Control as Speed Increases , 2005, Journal of motor behavior.

[161]  J. Buchanan,et al.  Scaling Movement Amplitude: Adaptation of Timing and Amplitude Control in a Bimanual Task , 2012, Journal of motor behavior.

[162]  I. Robertson,et al.  Rehabilitation of brain damage: brain plasticity and principles of guided recovery. , 1999, Psychological bulletin.

[163]  J. Kelso,et al.  Posturally induced transitions in rhythmic multijoint limb movements , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[164]  S. P. Swinnen,et al.  Relative phase destabilization during interlimb coordination: the disruptive role of kinesthetic afferences induced by passive movement , 1990, Experimental Brain Research.