No evidence for an attentional bias towards implicit temporal regularities
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] A. Clark. A nice surprise? Predictive processing and the active pursuit of novelty , 2018 .
[2] Anna C. Nobre,et al. Anticipated moments: temporal structure in attention , 2017, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[3] H. Rijn,et al. Pupillary response indexes the metrical hierarchy of unattended rhythmic violations , 2017, Brain and Cognition.
[4] B. Hayden,et al. The Psychology and Neuroscience of Curiosity , 2015, Neuron.
[5] Jeffrey N. Rouder,et al. Computation of Bayes Factors for Common Designs , 2015 .
[6] Ru Qi Yu,et al. The persistence of the attentional bias to regularities in a changing environment , 2015, Attention, perception & psychophysics.
[7] Sander Martens,et al. Training-induced Changes in the Dynamics of Attention as Reflected in Pupil Dilation , 2015, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[8] D. Berlyne. Conflict, arousal, and curiosity , 2014 .
[9] Richard N Aslin,et al. The Goldilocks effect in infant auditory attention. , 2014, Child development.
[10] D. Bates,et al. Fitting Linear Mixed-Effects Models Using lme4 , 2014, 1406.5823.
[11] Jennifer T. Coull,et al. Metrical Rhythm Implicitly Orients Attention in Time as Indexed by Improved Target Detection and Left Inferior Parietal Activation , 2014, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[12] R Core Team,et al. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. , 2014 .
[13] Daniel Ying-Jeh Little,et al. Maximal mutual information, not minimal entropy, for escaping the "Dark Room". , 2013, The Behavioral and brain sciences.
[14] N. Turk-Browne,et al. Attention Is Spontaneously Biased Toward Regularities , 2013, Psychological science.
[15] J. Obleser,et al. Frequency modulation entrains slow neural oscillations and optimizes human listening behavior , 2012, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[16] Virginie van Wassenhove,et al. Temporal Structure in Audiovisual Sensory Selection , 2012, PloS one.
[17] Richard N. Aslin,et al. The Goldilocks Effect: Human Infants Allocate Attention to Visual Sequences That Are Neither Too Simple Nor Too Complex , 2012, PloS one.
[18] Diane M. Beck,et al. Rescuing stimuli from invisibility: Inducing a momentary release from visual masking with pre-target entrainment , 2010, Cognition.
[19] Marvin M. Chun,et al. Neural Evidence of Statistical Learning: Efficient Detection of Visual Regularities Without Awareness , 2009, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[20] C. Schroeder,et al. Low-frequency neuronal oscillations as instruments of sensory selection , 2009, Trends in Neurosciences.
[21] George A. Alvarez,et al. Overwriting and rebinding: Why feature-switch detection tasks underestimate the binding capacity of visual working memory , 2009 .
[22] Marvin M. Chun,et al. Babies and Brains: Habituation in Infant Cognition and Functional Neuroimaging , 2008, Frontiers in human neuroscience.
[23] Richard D. Morey,et al. Confidence Intervals from Normalized Data: A correction to Cousineau (2005) , 2008 .
[24] G. Karmos,et al. Entrainment of Neuronal Oscillations as a Mechanism of Attentional Selection , 2008, Science.
[25] Denis G. Pelli,et al. ECVP '07 Abstracts , 2007, Perception.
[26] B. Scholl,et al. The Automaticity of Visual Statistical Learning Statistical Learning , 2005 .
[27] J. Lupiáñez,et al. Attentional preparation based on temporal expectancy modulates processing at the perceptual level , 2005, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[28] S. Martens,et al. Timing attention: Cuing target onset interval attenuates the attentional blink , 2005, Memory & cognition.
[29] G. Recanzone. Auditory influences on visual temporal rate perception. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.
[30] Bruno H. Repp,et al. Auditory dominance in temporal processing: new evidence from synchronization with simultaneous visual and auditory sequences. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[31] Aniruddh D. Patel,et al. Spectral decomposition of variability in synchronization and continuation tapping: comparisons between auditory and visual pacing and feedback conditions. , 2002, Human movement science.
[32] M. Jones,et al. Temporal Aspects of Stimulus-Driven Attending in Dynamic Arrays , 2002, Psychological science.
[33] G. Woodman,et al. Voluntary and automatic attentional control of visual working memory , 2002 .
[34] Edward W. Large,et al. Perceiving temporal regularity in music , 2002, Cogn. Sci..
[35] R. Aslin,et al. PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Article UNSUPERVISED STATISTICAL LEARNING OF HIGHER-ORDER SPATIAL STRUCTURES FROM VISUAL SCENES , 2022 .
[36] M. Chun,et al. Temporal contextual cuing of visual attention. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[37] E. Large,et al. The dynamics of attending: How people track time-varying events. , 1999 .
[38] M. Chun,et al. Contextual Cueing: Implicit Learning and Memory of Visual Context Guides Spatial Attention , 1998, Cognitive Psychology.
[39] D H Brainard,et al. The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.
[40] R N Aslin,et al. Statistical Learning by 8-Month-Old Infants , 1996, Science.
[41] P A Kolers,et al. Rhythms and responses. , 1985, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[42] S. Yantis,et al. Abrupt visual onsets and selective attention: evidence from visual search. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[43] I. Biederman,et al. Scene perception: Detecting and judging objects undergoing relational violations , 1982, Cognitive Psychology.
[44] D J Povel,et al. Internal representation of simple temporal patterns. , 1981, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[45] R. Näätänen,et al. Foreperiod and simple reaction time. , 1981 .
[46] A. Reber. Implicit learning of artificial grammars , 1967 .
[47] W. N. Dember,et al. Analysis of exploratory, manipulatory, and curiosity behaviors. , 1957, Psychological review.