Attentional Networks Functioning, Age, and Attentional Lapses While Driving

Objective: Based on Posner's (1994) model of attentional functions, the relationship between age and personal proneness to attention-related errors while driving and the functioning of the 3 attentional networks were explored by means of attentional behavioral measures and self-report data. Methods: A sample of 55 drivers was drawn from the general population of Mar del Plata, Buenos Aires, Argentina. The Attention Network Test for Interactions (ANTI) (Callejas et al. 2004) task was used to obtain behavioral measures of the attentional networks, and we used the Attention-Related Driving Error Scale (ARDES) (Ledesma et al. 2010) questionnaire to obtain a self-report measure of attention-related driving errors. Results: Drivers reporting the greatest propensity to experience attention-related errors showed an overall slowdown in performance, less endogenous preparation for high = priority warning signs, and a better response to conflict in the presence of valid cues than drivers less prone to attention-related errors while driving. Older participants showed a slowdown in performance, less endogenous preparation for high-priority warning signs, and worse cognitive control when solving conflict in comparison with younger drivers. Conclusions: We suggest that each group variable, attention-related error proneness and age, shows a particular combination of attentional network functioning that implies different ways of being distracted, which have different practical implications for safe driving. It can be inferred that drivers who are more prone to commit attentional errors while driving run less risk in situations in which they can deal with response conflict in the presence of valid cues because of the particular way in which their attentional networks are combined when a valid cue is present, could serve to compensate, with a better response conflict, their general slowdown and less endogenous preparedness for high-priority signs. It can be inferred that older drivers might show a reduction of the general state of alertness to signs that indicate the presence of a dangerous driving situation and in solving conflict in traffic circumstances.

[1]  J. Lupiáñez,et al.  Measuring vigilance while assessing the functioning of the three attentional networks: The ANTI-Vigilance task , 2011, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[2]  Eric Rodgman,et al.  The role of driver distraction in traffic crashes , 2001 .

[3]  J. Yager,et al.  Stroke in Children and Young Adults , 1995 .

[4]  Pío Tudela,et al.  Modulations among the alerting, orienting and executive control networks , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.

[5]  Raymond M. Klein,et al.  Are Individual Differences in Absentmindedness Correlated with Individual Differences in Attention , 2009 .

[6]  Juan Lupiáñez,et al.  The three attentional networks: On their independence and interactions , 2004, Brain and Cognition.

[7]  K. Ball,et al.  Visual attention problems as a predictor of vehicle crashes in older drivers. , 1993, Investigative ophthalmology & visual science.

[8]  Pío Tudela,et al.  The attentional mechanism of temporal orienting: determinants and attributes , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.

[9]  Jonathan S. A. Carriere,et al.  Absent-mindedness: Lapses of conscious awareness and everyday cognitive failures , 2006, Consciousness and Cognition.

[10]  Karlene K. Ball,et al.  Reliability and Validity of Useful Field of View Test Scores as Administered by Personal Computer , 2005, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[11]  Liisa Hakamies-Blomqvist,et al.  RESEARCH ON OLDER DRIVERS: A REVIEW , 1996 .

[12]  E. Bernstein,et al.  Development, reliability, and validity of a dissociation scale. , 1986, The Journal of nervous and mental disease.

[14]  T A Ramney,et al.  Driver distraction: a review of the current state-of-the-knowledge , 2008 .

[15]  D. Broadbent,et al.  The Cognitive Failures Questionnaire (CFQ) and its correlates. , 1982, The British journal of clinical psychology.

[16]  R. Farmer,et al.  Boredom proneness--the development and correlates of a new scale. , 1986, Journal of personality assessment.

[17]  Dale Dagenbach,et al.  Age-Related Changes and the Attention Network Task: An Examination of Alerting, Orienting, and Executive Function , 2007, Neuropsychology, development, and cognition. Section B, Aging, neuropsychology and cognition.

[18]  Bruce D. McCandliss,et al.  Testing the Efficiency and Independence of Attentional Networks , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[19]  J. Lupiáñez,et al.  Cognitive-behavioral therapy for insomnia improves attentional function in fibromyalgia syndrome: A pilot, randomized controlled trial , 2011, Journal of health psychology.

[20]  J. Lupiáñez,et al.  Attentional deficits in fibromyalgia and its relationships with pain, emotional distress and sleep dysfunction complaints , 2011, Psychology & health.

[21]  John D. Lee,et al.  Sources of Driver Distraction , 2009 .

[22]  Jonathan S. A. Carriere,et al.  Everyday attention lapses and memory failures: The affective consequences of mindlessness , 2008, Consciousness and Cognition.

[23]  J. Enns,et al.  Paying attention behind the wheel: a framework for studying the role of attention in driving , 2004 .

[24]  S Stradling,et al.  Errors and violations on the roads: a real distinction? , 1990, Ergonomics.

[25]  Michel Bédard,et al.  Using the Attention Network Test to predict driving test scores. , 2009, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[26]  M. Posner Attention: the mechanisms of consciousness. , 1994, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[27]  D. Roth,et al.  Cumulative Meta-analysis of the Relationship Between Useful Field of View and Driving Performance in Older Adults: Current and Future Implications , 2005, Optometry and vision science : official publication of the American Academy of Optometry.

[28]  Graham J Hole,et al.  Some factors affecting motorcyclists' conspicuity , 1996 .

[29]  Paul Verhaeghen,et al.  Cognitive efficiency modes in old age: performance on sequential and coordinative verbal and visuospatial tasks. , 2002, Psychology and aging.

[30]  Trent Victor,et al.  Distraction Assessment Methods Based on Visual Behavior and Event Detection , 2009 .

[31]  Dot Hs Driver Distraction: A Review of the Current State-of-Knowledge , 2008 .

[32]  M. Posner,et al.  The attention system of the human brain. , 1990, Annual review of neuroscience.

[33]  T. Salthouse The processing-speed theory of adult age differences in cognition. , 1996, Psychological review.

[34]  Eric Rodgman,et al.  Driver's exposure to distractions in their natural driving environment. , 2005, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[35]  Rubén D Ledesma,et al.  Individual Differences in Driver Inattention: The Attention-Related Driving Errors Scale , 2010, Traffic injury prevention.

[36]  Graham J Hole,et al.  The Psychology of Driving , 2006 .

[37]  A. Muzet,et al.  Oculomotor changes due to road events during prolonged monotonous simulated driving , 2005, Biological Psychology.

[38]  J. Toby Mordkoff,et al.  Chronotype and time-of-day influences on the alerting, orienting, and executive components of attention , 2008, Experimental Brain Research.

[39]  M Oström,et al.  Role of disease in initiating the crashes of fatally injured drivers. , 1996, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[40]  Michael A. Regan,et al.  Driver distraction: A review of the literature , 2003 .

[41]  M. Mikulincer,et al.  The multidimensional driving style inventory--scale construct and validation. , 2004, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[42]  J. Cerella Information processing rates in the elderly. , 1985, Psychological bulletin.

[43]  R. Ryan,et al.  The benefits of being present: mindfulness and its role in psychological well-being. , 2003, Journal of personality and social psychology.