Asynchrony in the cognitive and lexical development of young children with Williams syndrome

The present study investigates whether five-to-six-year-old children with Williams syndrome (N=8) can form new object categories based on naming information alone, and compares them with five groups of typically developing children aged 2;0 to 6;0 (N=34 children). Children were presented with triads of dissimilar objects; all objects in a triad were labelled, two of them with the same pseudoname. Name-based categorization was evaluated through object selection. Performance was above chance level for all groups. Performance reached a ceiling at about 4;0 for the typically developing children. For the children with Williams Syndrome, performance remained below chronological age level. The present results are discussed in light of previous findings of a failure to perform name-based categorization in younger children with Williams syndrome and the persistent asynchrony between cognitive and lexical development in this disorder.

[1]  Julia Grant,et al.  Comprehension of Spatial Language Terms in Williams Syndrome: Evidence for an Interaction Between Domains of Strength and Weakness , 2004, Cortex.

[2]  Michael S. C. Thomas,et al.  Exploring the Williams syndrome face-processing debate: the importance of building developmental trajectories. , 2004, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[3]  A. Karmiloff-Smith,et al.  Early Word Segmentation by Infants and Toddlers With Williams Syndrome , 2003 .

[4]  M. C. Caselli,et al.  Early Linguistic Abilities of Italian Children With Williams Syndrome , 2003, Developmental neuropsychology.

[5]  M. C. Caselli,et al.  Language acquisition in special populations: a comparison between Down and Williams syndromes , 2002, Neuropsychologia.

[6]  A. Karmiloff-Smith,et al.  Early categorization abilities in young children with Williams syndrome , 2002, Neuroreport.

[7]  Daniel Ansari,et al.  Atypical development of language and social communication in toddlers with Williams syndrome , 2002 .

[8]  Virginia Valian,et al.  A study of relative clauses in Williams syndrome , 2002, Journal of Child Language.

[9]  A. Gopnik,et al.  Linguistic and cognitive abilities in infancy: when does language become a tool for categorization? , 2001, Cognition.

[10]  Laura Lakusta,et al.  Past tense formation in Williams syndrome , 2001 .

[11]  A. Karmiloff-Smith,et al.  Williams syndrome: from genotype through to the cognitive phenotype. , 2000, American journal of medical genetics.

[12]  A. Karmiloff-Smith,et al.  Cognitive modularity and genetic disorders. , 1999, Science.

[13]  Mary R. Newsome,et al.  The Beginnings of Word Segmentation in English-Learning Infants , 1999, Cognitive Psychology.

[14]  A. Karmiloff-Smith Development itself is the key to understanding developmental disorders , 1998, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[15]  A. Karmiloff-Smith,et al.  Word learning in a special population: do individuals with Williams syndrome obey lexical constraints? , 1997, Journal of Child Language.

[16]  J. Pind The Discovery of Spoken Language, Peter W. Jusczyk (Ed.). MIT Press (1997), ISBN 0 262 10058 4 , 1997 .

[17]  A. Karmiloff-Smith,et al.  Phonological Short-term Memory and its Relationship to Language in Williams Syndrome. , 1997, Cognitive neuropsychiatry.

[18]  Virginia Volterra,et al.  Memory Abilities in Children with Williams Syndrome , 1996, Cortex.

[19]  G. Carlesimo,et al.  Short-term memory in children with Williams syndrome: A reduced contribution of lexical-semantic knowledge to word span , 1996, Neuropsychologia.

[20]  M. Tassabehji,et al.  LIM–kinase deleted in Williams syndrome , 1996, Nature Genetics.

[21]  E. Clark JCL volume 22 issue 1 Cover and Back matter , 1995, Journal of Child Language.

[22]  C. Mervis,et al.  Acquisition of the novel name--nameless category (N3C) principle. , 1994, Child development.

[23]  Pierre A. Hallé,et al.  Emergence of an early receptive lexicon: Infants' recognition of words , 1994 .

[24]  Patricia Spallone,et al.  Hemizygosity at the elastin locus in a developmental disorder, Williams syndrome , 1993, Nature Genetics.

[25]  Alison Gopnik,et al.  Categorization and Naming: Basic-Level Sorting in Eighteen-Month-Olds and Its Relation to Language , 1992 .

[26]  H. Benedict,et al.  Early lexical development: comprehension and production , 1979, Journal of Child Language.

[27]  S. Waxman Links between object categorization and naming: Origins and emergence in human infants , 2003 .

[28]  J. Werker,et al.  Infants' Ability to Learn Phonetically Similar Words: Effects of Age and Vocabulary Size , 2002 .

[29]  Rebecca J. Brand,et al.  Breaking the language barrier: an emergentist coalition model for the origins of word learning. , 2000, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.

[30]  Carolyn B. Mervis,et al.  Williams syndrome: Findings from an integrated program of research , 1999 .

[31]  Ursula Bellugi,et al.  Contrasting profiles of language development in children with williams and down syndromes , 1997 .

[32]  A. Karmiloff-Smith,et al.  Is the language of people with Williams syndrome mere mimicry ? Phonological short-term memory in a foreign language , 1996 .

[33]  E. Bates,et al.  From First Words to Grammar: Individual Differences and Dissociable Mechanisms , 1988 .