Transfer of calibration in length perception by dynamic touch

Earlier studies suggested that the calibration of actions is functionally, rather than anatomically, specific; thus, calibration of an action ought to transfer to actions that serve the same goal (Rieser, Pick, Ashmead, & Garing, 1995). In the present study, we investigated whether the calibration of perception also follows a functional organization: If one means of detecting an information variable is recalibrated, are other means of detection recalibrated as well? In two experiments, visual feedback was used to recalibrate perceived length of a rod wielded by the right hand; the recalibration was found to transfer to length perception with the left hand. This implies that calibration in perception is organized functionally rather than anatomically, and supports the general view that calibration applies to functional systems.

[1]  E. Gibson,et al.  The effect of training on absolute estimation of distance over the ground. , 1954, Journal of experimental psychology.

[2]  Heft Harry,et al.  A Methodological Note on Overestimates of Reaching Distance: Distinguishing Between Perceptual and Analytical Judgments , 1993 .

[3]  B Wallace,et al.  Additivity in prism adaptation as manifested in intermanual and interocular transfer , 1979, Perception & psychophysics.

[4]  Michael T. Turvey,et al.  Effortful touch with minimal movement , 1992 .

[5]  M M Cohen,et al.  Continuous versus Terminal Visual Feedback in Prism Aftereffects , 1967, Perceptual and motor skills.

[6]  I. Kingma,et al.  The Inertia Tensor Versus Static Moment and Mass in Perceiving Length and Heaviness of Hand-Wielded Rods , 2002 .

[7]  G P Bingham,et al.  Distortions in definite distance and shape perception as measured by reaching without and with haptic feedback. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[8]  J. Gibson The Senses Considered As Perceptual Systems , 1967 .

[9]  Anthony M. Avolio,et al.  Walking infants adapt locomotion to changing body dimensions. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[10]  Stuart Anstis,et al.  Aftereffects from jogging , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[11]  K. Lashley Basic neural mechanisms in behavior. , 1930 .

[12]  F. Attneave,et al.  The Organization of Behavior: A Neuropsychological Theory , 1949 .

[13]  E. Reed Encountering the world: Toward an ecological psychology. , 1997 .

[14]  K. Maccorquodale Organization of Behavior : A Neuropsychological Theory , 1951 .

[15]  J. Gibson The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception , 1979 .

[16]  E. Reed Chapter 21 – Becoming A Self 1 , 1995 .

[17]  Gordon M. Redding,et al.  Adaptive Spatial Alignment , 1997 .

[18]  A. Luria The Working Brain , 1973 .

[19]  Rob Withagen,et al.  The Calibration of Walking Transfers to Crawling: Are Action Systems Calibrated? , 2002 .

[20]  C R HAMILTON,et al.  INTERMANUAL TRANSFER OF ADAPTATION TO PRISMS. , 1964, The American journal of psychology.

[21]  Anne E. Garing,et al.  Calibration of human locomotion and models of perceptual-motor organization. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[22]  M. Turvey,et al.  Inertial constraints on limb proprioception are independent of visual calibration. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[23]  C. S. Harris Adaptation to Displaced Vision: Visual, Motor, or Proprioceptive Change? , 1963, Science.

[24]  E. Reed An outline of a theory of action systems. , 1982, Journal of motor behavior.

[25]  B Wallace,et al.  Components of prism adaptation in terminal and concurrent exposure: Organization of the eye-hand coordination loop , 1988, Perception & psychophysics.

[26]  M T Turvey,et al.  Haptically perceiving the distances reachable with hand-held objects. , 1988, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[27]  Vittorio Gallese,et al.  A neuroscientific grasp of concepts: from control to representation. , 2003, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[28]  L. S. Mark,et al.  Eyeheight-scaled information about affordances: a study of sitting and stair climbing. , 1987, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[29]  James E. Cutting,et al.  Perception with an eye for motion , 1986 .

[30]  James E. Cutting,et al.  Four Ways to Reject Directed Perception , 1991 .

[31]  Geoffrey P. Bingham,et al.  Task-specific devices and the perceptual bottleneck☆ , 1988 .

[32]  M T Turvey,et al.  Tensorial basis to the constancy of perceived object extent over variations of dynamic touch , 1993, Perception & psychophysics.

[33]  S. Flynn The Perception of Relative Mass in Physical Collisions , 1994 .

[34]  C. S. Harris Perceptual adaptation to inverted, reversed, and displaced vision. , 1965, Psychological review.

[35]  P. N. Kugler,et al.  1 On the Concept of Coordinative Structures as Dissipative Structures: I. Theoretical Lines of Convergence* , 1980 .

[36]  Michael T. Turvey,et al.  Chapter 11 – Dynamic Touch , 1995 .

[37]  M. Turvey,et al.  Eigenvalues of the inertia tensor and exteroception by the “muscular sense” , 1994, Neuroscience.

[38]  M T Turvey,et al.  Attunement, Calibration, and Exploration in Fast Haptic Perceptual Learning , 2001, Journal of motor behavior.