Dummy eye measurements of microsaccades: Testing the influence of system noise and head movements on microsaccade detection in a popular video-based eye tracker

Whereas early studies of microsaccades have predominantly relied on custom-built eye trackers and manual tagging of microsaccades, more recent work tends to use video-based eye tracking and automated algorithms for microsaccade detection. While data from these newer studies suggest that microsaccades can be reliably detected with video-based systems, this has not been systematically evaluated. I here present a method and data examining microsaccade detection in an often used video-based system (the Eyelink II system) and a commonly used detection algorithm (Engbert & Kliegl, 2003; Engbert & Mergenthaler, 2006). Recordings from human participants and those obtained using a pair of dummy eyes, mounted on a pair of glasses either worn by a human participant (i.e., with head motion) or a dummy head (no head motion) were compared. Three experiments were conducted. The first experiment suggests that when microsaccade measurements make use of the pupil detection mode, microsaccade detections in the absence of eye movements are sparse in the absence of head movements, but frequent with head movements (despite the use of a chin rest). A second experiment demonstrates that by using measurements that rely on a combination of corneal reflection and pupil detection, false microsaccade detections can be largely avoided as long as a binocular criterion is used. A third experiment examines whether past results may have been affected by possible incorrect detections due to small head movements. It shows that despite the many detections due to head movements, the typical modulation of microsaccade rate after stimulus onset is found only when recording from the participants’ eyes.

[1]  Robin Walker,et al.  Gaze Stability of Observers Watching Op Art Pictures , 2003, Perception.

[2]  Nikos K. Logothetis,et al.  The Distribution of Microsaccade Directions Need Not Reveal the Location of Attention: Reply to Rolfs, Engbert, and Kliegl , 2004 .

[3]  H. Collewijn,et al.  The significance of microsaccades for vision and oculomotor control. , 2008, Journal of vision.

[4]  M. Rolfs Microsaccades: Small steps on a long way , 2009, Vision Research.

[5]  Ralf Engbert,et al.  Microsaccade Orientation Supports Attentional Enhancement Opposite a Peripheral Cue: Commentary on Tse, Sheinberg, and Logothetis (2003) , 2004, Psychological science.

[6]  D. Hubel,et al.  Microsaccadic eye movements and firing of single cells in the striate cortex of macaque monkeys , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[7]  Robin Walker,et al.  What determines the direction of microsaccades , 2010 .

[8]  Ralf Engbert,et al.  When do microsaccades follow spatial attention? , 2010, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[9]  J. Wolfe,et al.  Fixational Eye Movements Are Not an Index of Covert Attention , 2007, Psychological science.

[10]  Ralf Engbert,et al.  Microsaccades uncover the orientation of covert attention , 2003, Vision Research.

[11]  Brian D Corneil,et al.  Neuromuscular consequences of reflexive covert orienting , 2008, Nature Neuroscience.

[12]  Xoana G. Troncoso,et al.  Microsaccades: a neurophysiological analysis , 2009, Trends in Neurosciences.

[13]  R. Steinman,et al.  Voluntary Control of Microsaccades during Maintained Monocular Fixation , 1967, Science.

[14]  Ralf Engbert,et al.  Microsaccade dynamics during covert attention , 2005, Vision Research.

[15]  Carlos Hitoshi Morimoto,et al.  Eye gaze tracking techniques for interactive applications , 2005, Comput. Vis. Image Underst..

[16]  R. Steinman Effect of Target Size, Luminance, and Color on Monocular Fixation* , 1965 .

[17]  B. L. Zuber,et al.  Microsaccades and the Velocity-Amplitude Relationship for Saccadic Eye Movements , 1965, Science.

[18]  F. Møller,et al.  Binocular quantification and characterization of microsaccades , 2002, Graefe’s Archive for Clinical and Experimental Ophthalmology.

[19]  Ralf Engbert,et al.  Microsaccades are triggered by low retinal image slip. , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[20]  R. Cohen,et al.  Directional Bias of Limb Tremor Prior to Voluntary Movement , 2007, Psychological science.

[21]  William T. Newsome,et al.  Tracking the eye non-invasively: simultaneous comparison of the scleral search coil and optical tracking techniques in the macaque monkey , 2012, Front. Behav. Neurosci..

[22]  James J. Clark,et al.  Microsaccades as an overt measure of covert attention shifts , 2002, Vision Research.

[23]  Ralf Engbert,et al.  Microsaccades are different from saccades in scene perception , 2010, Experimental Brain Research.

[24]  A. Sjølie,et al.  The contribution of microsaccades and drifts in the maintenance of binocular steady fixation , 2006, Graefe's Archive for Clinical and Experimental Ophthalmology.

[25]  J. Braun,et al.  Rare but precious: Microsaccades are highly informative about attentional allocation , 2010, Vision Research.

[26]  Maik C. Stüttgen,et al.  Computation of measures of effect size for neuroscience data sets , 2011, The European journal of neuroscience.

[27]  Ralf Engbert,et al.  Microsaccades Are an Index of Covert Attention , 2007, Psychological science.

[28]  Stephen L Macknik,et al.  Unsupervised clustering method to detect microsaccades. , 2014, Journal of vision.

[29]  Jeremy M. Wolfe,et al.  Microsaccades and Attention: Does a Weak Correlation Make an Index? , 2007 .

[30]  David L. Sheinberg,et al.  Fixational eye movements are not affected by abrupt onsets that capture attention , 2002, Vision Research.

[31]  Ralf Engbert Microsaccades: A microcosm for research on oculomotor control, attention, and visual perception. , 2006, Progress in brain research.

[32]  D. Hubel,et al.  The function of bursts of spikes during visual fixation in the awake primate lateral geniculate nucleus and primary visual cortex , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.