Who Falls for Fake News? The Roles of Bullshit Receptivity, Overclaiming, Familiarity, and Analytic Thinking

OBJECTIVE Fake news represents a particularly egregious and direct avenue by which inaccurate beliefs have been propagated via social media. We investigate the psychological profile of individuals who fall prey to fake news. METHOD We recruited 1,606 participants from Amazon's Mechanical Turk for three online surveys. RESULTS The tendency to ascribe profundity to randomly generated sentences-pseudo-profound bullshit receptivity-correlates positively with perceptions of fake news accuracy, and negatively with the ability to differentiate between fake and real news (media truth discernment). Relatedly, individuals who overclaim their level of knowledge also judge fake news to be more accurate. We also extend previous research indicating that analytic thinking correlates negatively with perceived accuracy by showing that this relationship is not moderated by the presence/absence of the headline's source (which has no effect on accuracy), or by familiarity with the headlines (which correlates positively with perceived accuracy of fake and real news). CONCLUSION Our results suggest that belief in fake news may be driven, to some extent, by a general tendency to be overly accepting of weak claims. This tendency, which we refer to as reflexive open-mindedness, may be partly responsible for the prevalence of epistemically suspect beliefs writ large.

[1]  John Staddon What Is Rational? , 2021 .

[2]  Jonathan A. Busam,et al.  Real Solutions for Fake News? Measuring the Effectiveness of General Warnings and Fact-Check Tags in Reducing Belief in False Stories on Social Media , 2020, Political Behavior.

[3]  Silvia Knobloch-Westerwick,et al.  Confirmation Bias, Ingroup Bias, and Negativity Bias in Selective Exposure to Political Information , 2020 .

[4]  David G. Rand,et al.  The Implied Truth Effect: Attaching Warnings to a Subset of Fake News Headlines Increases Perceived Accuracy of Headlines Without Warnings , 2019, Manag. Sci..

[5]  J. Baron Actively open-minded thinking in politics , 2019, Cognition.

[6]  David G. Rand,et al.  Lazy, not biased: Susceptibility to partisan fake news is better explained by lack of reasoning than by motivated reasoning , 2019, Cognition.

[7]  David G. Rand,et al.  Investigating the Robustness of the Illusory Truth Effect Across Individual Differences in Cognitive Ability, Need for Cognitive Closure, and Cognitive Style , 2019, Personality & social psychology bulletin.

[8]  David G. Rand,et al.  Repetition increases perceived truth equally for plausible and implausible statements , 2019, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.

[9]  Jonathan A. Busam,et al.  Real Solutions for Fake News? Measuring the Effectiveness of General Warnings and Fact-Check Tags in Reducing Belief in False Stories on Social Media , 2019, Political Behavior.

[10]  D. Lazer,et al.  Fake news on Twitter during the 2016 U.S. presidential election , 2019, Science.

[11]  Joshua A. Tucker,et al.  Less than you think: Prevalence and predictors of fake news dissemination on Facebook , 2019, Science Advances.

[12]  David G. Rand,et al.  Belief in Fake News Is Associated with Delusionality, Dogmatism, Religious Fundamentalism, and Reduced Analytic Thinking , 2018, Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition.

[13]  David G. Rand,et al.  Prior Exposure Increases Perceived Accuracy of Fake News , 2018, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[14]  John V. Petrocelli,et al.  Antecedents of bullshitting , 2018 .

[15]  David G. Rand,et al.  Is the Illusory Truth Effect Robust to Individual Differences in Cognitive Ability, Need for Cognitive Closure, and Cognitive Style? , 2018 .

[16]  Gordon Pennycook The New Reflectionism in Cognitive Psychology : Why Reason Matters , 2018 .

[17]  Miriam J. Metzger,et al.  The science of fake news , 2018, Science.

[18]  Sinan Aral,et al.  The spread of true and false news online , 2018, Science.

[19]  David G. Rand,et al.  Fighting misinformation on social media using crowdsourced judgments of news source quality , 2018, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[20]  K. Jamieson,et al.  Processing the papal encyclical through perceptual filters: Pope Francis, identity-protective cognition, and climate change concern , 2017, Cognition.

[21]  Caitlin Drummond,et al.  Individuals with greater science literacy and education have more polarized beliefs on controversial science topics , 2017, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[22]  Onurcan Yilmaz,et al.  Activating analytic thinking enhances the value given to individualizing moral foundations , 2017, Cognition.

[23]  Stephan Lewandowsky,et al.  Reminders and repetition of misinformation: Helping or hindering its retraction? , 2017, Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition.

[24]  D. Kahan Misconceptions, Misinformation, and the Logic of Identity-Protective Cognition , 2017 .

[25]  Ullrich K. H. Ecker,et al.  The Role of Familiarity in Correcting Inaccurate Information , 2017, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[26]  Paul Slovic,et al.  Motivated numeracy and enlightened self-government , 2017, Behavioural Public Policy.

[27]  David G. Rand,et al.  Turking overtime: how participant characteristics and behavior vary over time and day on Amazon Mechanical Turk , 2017, Journal of the Economic Science Association.

[28]  Adam J. Berinsky,et al.  Processing political misinformation: comprehending the Trump phenomenon , 2017, Royal Society Open Science.

[29]  Jonathan A. Fugelsang,et al.  Dunning–Kruger effects in reasoning: Theoretical implications of the failure to recognize incompetence , 2017, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[30]  David G. Rand,et al.  Why Do We Hate Hypocrites? Evidence for a Theory of False Signaling , 2017, Psychological science.

[31]  M. Gentzkow,et al.  Social Media and Fake News in the 2016 Election , 2017 .

[32]  Arnaud Legout,et al.  Social Clicks: What and Who Gets Read on Twitter? , 2016, SIGMETRICS.

[33]  Roberto Cabeza,et al.  On Known Unknowns: Fluency and the Neural Mechanisms of Illusory Truth , 2016, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[34]  Jonathan A. Fugelsang,et al.  Atheists and Agnostics Are More Reflective than Religious Believers: Four Empirical Studies and a Meta-Analysis , 2016, PloS one.

[35]  Gordon Pennycook,et al.  Commentary: Cognitive reflection vs. calculation in decision making , 2016, Front. Psychol..

[36]  Daniel M. Oppenheimer,et al.  Investigating an alternate form of the cognitive reflection test , 2016, Judgment and Decision Making.

[37]  M. Walsh,et al.  The Role of Shear Stress in Arteriovenous Fistula Maturation and Failure: A Systematic Review , 2015, PloS one.

[38]  Jonathan A. Fugelsang,et al.  Everyday Consequences of Analytic Thinking , 2015 .

[39]  Jonathan A. Fugelsang,et al.  On the reception and detection of pseudo-profound bullshit , 2015, Judgment and Decision Making.

[40]  Sydney E. Scott,et al.  Why does the Cognitive Reflection Test (sometimes) predict utilitarian moral judgment (and other things) , 2015 .

[41]  Gordon Pennycook,et al.  Going against the Herd: Psychological and Cultural Factors Underlying the ‘Vaccination Confidence Gap’ , 2015, PloS one.

[42]  W. Gervais Override the controversy: Analytic thinking predicts endorsement of evolution , 2015, Cognition.

[43]  Nadia M. Brashier,et al.  Knowledge does not protect against illusory truth. , 2015, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[44]  Jonathan A. Fugelsang,et al.  What makes us think? A three-stage dual-process model of analytic engagement , 2015, Cognitive Psychology.

[45]  Jonathan A. Fugelsang,et al.  The brain in your pocket: Evidence that Smartphones are used to supplant thinking , 2015, Comput. Hum. Behav..

[46]  Adam J. Berinsky,et al.  Rumors and Health Care Reform: Experiments in Political Misinformation , 2015, British Journal of Political Science.

[47]  Jean‐François Bonnefon,et al.  Non-Reflective Thinkers Are Predisposed to Attribute Supernatural Causation to Uncanny Experiences , 2015, Personality & social psychology bulletin.

[48]  Jonathan A. Fugelsang,et al.  Is the cognitive reflection test a measure of both reflection and intuition? , 2015, Behavior Research Methods.

[49]  Edward B. Royzman,et al.  Are Thoughtful People More Utilitarian? CRT as a Unique Predictor of Moral Minimalism in the Dilemmatic Context , 2015, Cogn. Sci..

[50]  A. Furnham,et al.  Analytic thinking reduces belief in conspiracy theories , 2014, Cognition.

[51]  Geoffrey P. Goodwin,et al.  Are good reasoners more incest-friendly? Trait cognitive reflection predicts selective moralization in a sample of American adults , 2014, Judgment and Decision Making.

[52]  Guillermo Campitelli,et al.  Does the cognitive reflection test measure cognitive reflection? A mathematical modeling approach , 2014, Memory & cognition.

[53]  Keith E. Stanovich,et al.  Assessing miserly information processing: An expansion of the Cognitive Reflection Test , 2014 .

[54]  Dan M. Kahan,et al.  Ideology, motivated reasoning, and cognitive reflection , 2013, Judgment and Decision Making.

[55]  Jonathan A. Fugelsang,et al.  Cognitive style and religiosity: The role of conflict detection , 2013, Memory & Cognition.

[56]  Ilana Ritov,et al.  The role of actively open-minded thinking in information acquisition, accuracy, and calibration , 2013, Judgment and Decision Making.

[57]  Gregory N. Mandel,et al.  The Polarizing Impact of Science Literacy and Numeracy on Perceived Climate Change Risks , 2012 .

[58]  Jonathan A. Fugelsang,et al.  Analytic cognitive style predicts religious and paranormal belief , 2012, Cognition.

[59]  Will M. Gervais,et al.  Analytic Thinking Promotes Religious Disbelief , 2012, Science.

[60]  W. Neys Bias and Conflict A Case for Logical Intuitions , 2012 .

[61]  K. Stanovich,et al.  The Cognitive Reflection Test as a predictor of performance on heuristics-and-biases tasks , 2011, Memory & cognition.

[62]  Christoph Stahl,et al.  The Truth About the Truth: A Meta-Analytic Review of the Truth Effect , 2010, Personality and social psychology review : an official journal of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Inc.

[63]  B. Nyhan,et al.  When Corrections Fail: The Persistence of Political Misperceptions , 2010 .

[64]  Daniel M. Oppenheimer,et al.  Uniting the Tribes of Fluency to Form a Metacognitive Nation , 2009, Personality and social psychology review : an official journal of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Inc.

[65]  C. Frith Social cognition , 2008, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[66]  S. Frederick,et al.  Journal of Economic Perspectives—Volume 19, Number 4—Fall 2005—Pages 25–42 Cognitive Reflection and Decision Making , 2022 .

[67]  M. Nowak,et al.  Evolution of indirect reciprocity , 2005, Nature.

[68]  Chanthika Pornpitakpan The Persuasiveness of Source Credibility: A Critical Review of Five Decades' Evidence , 2004 .

[69]  D. Paulhus,et al.  The over-claiming technique: measuring self-enhancement independent of ability. , 2003, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[70]  N. Schwarz,et al.  Effects of Perceptual Fluency on Affective Judgments , 1998 .

[71]  S. Epstein,et al.  Individual differences in intuitive-experiential and analytical-rational thinking styles. , 1996, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[72]  J. Cacioppo,et al.  DISPOSITIONAL DIFFERENCES IN COGNITIVE MOTIVATION : THE LIFE AND TIMES OF INDIVIDUALS VARYING IN NEED FOR COGNITION , 1996 .

[73]  B. W. Whittlesea Illusions of familiarity. , 1993 .

[74]  Ian Begg,et al.  Dissociation of processes in belief: Source recollection, statement familiarity, and the illusion of truth , 1992 .

[75]  Thomas C. Toppino,et al.  Frequency and the Conference of Referential Validity. , 1977 .

[76]  Michael Wright,et al.  The brain in your pocket , 1973 .

[77]  C. Ballarini Reasons and the “ Motivated Numeracy Effect ” , 2017 .

[78]  Andrew Shtulman,et al.  Cognitive Reflection Predicts Science Understanding , 2014, CogSci.

[79]  Joseph M. Paxton,et al.  Reflection and Reasoning in Moral Judgment , 2012, Cogn. Sci..

[80]  David G. Rand,et al.  Journal of Experimental Psychology: General Divine Intuition: Cognitive Style Influences Belief in God , 2011 .

[81]  D. Kahneman Thinking, Fast and Slow , 2011 .

[82]  M. Leary,et al.  Handbook of individual differences in social behavior , 2009 .

[83]  Christian Unkelbach,et al.  Reversing the truth effect: learning the interpretation of processing fluency in judgments of truth. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[84]  K. Stanovich,et al.  Individual differences in reasoning: Implications for the rationality debate?-Open Peer Commentary-Gone with the wind: Individual differences in heuristics and biases undermine the implication of , 2000 .

[85]  Seymour Epstein,et al.  Individual Differences in Intuitive-Exp eriential and Analytical-Rational Thinking Styles , 1996 .

[86]  J. Cacioppo,et al.  The need for cognition. , 1982 .

[87]  Floyd H. Allport,et al.  Wartime rumors of waste and special privilege: why some people believe them. , 1945 .

[88]  Jonathan Evans,et al.  Science Perspectives on Psychological , 2022 .