A Partisan Schema for Political Information Processing

Based on their interest in politics and knowledge of political leaders, individuals are classified into three levels of partisan sophistication: (1) those scoring high in interest and knowledge (partisan schematics), (2) a middle group, and (3) those scoring low (partisan aschematics). In this experimental study, and consistent with findings from cognitive and social psychology, partisan schematics prove better able than partisan aschematics to classify campaign statements as either Republican or Democratic and to recall the policy stands taken by a fictitious congressman. Aschematics, at the other extreme, perform at no better than chance levels in either the recognition or recall of the congressman's policy statements. There are, however, liabilities to sophistication as well: Schematics demonstrate a “consistency bias” in recalling significantly more policy statements that are consistent with the congressman's party identification than are inconsistent with it. This “restructuring” of memory is especially pronounced among sophisticates, and reflects a serious bias in the processing of political information.

[1]  Fred I. Greenstein,et al.  Handbook of political science , 1975 .

[2]  Milton Lodge,et al.  Partisan and ideological belief systems: Do they differ? , 1985 .

[3]  U. Neisser Cognition and reality: principles and implications , 1976 .

[4]  Robert P. Abelson,et al.  Differences Between Belief and Knowledge Systems , 1979, Cogn. Sci..

[5]  H. Markus Self-schemata and processing information about the self. , 1977 .

[6]  Milton Lodge,et al.  The Breadth, Depth, and Utility of Class, Partisan, and Ideological Schemata * , 1985 .

[7]  Richard C. Anderson,et al.  Schooling and the Acquisition of Knowledge , 1978 .

[8]  Marvin Minsky,et al.  A framework for representing knowledge , 1974 .

[9]  P. Converse,et al.  The American voter , 1960 .

[10]  V. Key The State of the Discipline , 1958, American Political Science Review.

[11]  E. Rosch Cognitive Representations of Semantic Categories. , 1975 .

[12]  P. J. Conover,et al.  How People Organize the Political World: A Schematic Model , 1984 .

[13]  Warren E. Miller,et al.  Ideological Interpretations of Presidential Elections , 1979, American Political Science Review.

[14]  S. J. Thorson,et al.  Thematic memory for politics in children , 1983 .

[15]  Lynn Hasher,et al.  Is memory schematic , 1983 .

[16]  S. Fiske,et al.  The novice and the expert: Knowledge-based strategies in political cognition , 1983 .

[17]  Philip E. Converse,et al.  A Dynamic Simultaneous Equation Model of Electoral Choice , 1979, American Political Science Review.

[18]  M. Lodge,et al.  The meaning of party labels , 1980 .

[19]  D. Apter,et al.  Ideology and discontent , 1966 .

[20]  M. Lodge,et al.  The Difference between Attitudes and Nonattitudes in the Mass Public: Just Measurements?* , 1985 .

[21]  W. Bennett The Growth of Knowledge in Mass Belief Studies: An Epistemological Critique , 1977 .

[22]  P. J. Conover,et al.  The Origins and Meaning of Liberal/Conservative Self-Identifications. , 1981 .

[23]  Patrick Henry Winston,et al.  The psychology of computer vision , 1976, Pattern Recognit..

[24]  Roger C. Schank,et al.  Scripts, plans, goals and understanding: an inquiry into human knowledge structures , 1978 .

[25]  James A. Stimson Belief Systems: Constraint, Complexity, and the 1972 Election , 1975 .

[26]  Robert S. Erikson,et al.  The SRC Panel Data and Mass Political Attitudes , 1979, British Journal of Political Science.

[27]  Norman H. Nie,et al.  The Changing American Voter. , 1976 .

[28]  H A Simon,et al.  The behavioral and social sciences. , 1980, Science.

[29]  D. F. Davis,et al.  Mental Economy and Voter Rationality: The Informed Citizen Problem in Voting Research , 1984, The Journal of Politics.

[30]  Robert Axelrod,et al.  Schema Theory: An Information Processing Model of Perception and Cognition , 1973, American Political Science Review.