As the Crow Flies: Bias in Consumers' Map-Based Distance Judgments

Consumers make distance judgments when they decide which store to visit or which route to take. However, these judgments may be prone to various spatial perception biases. While there is a rich literature on spatial perceptions in urban planning and environmental and cognitive psychology, there is little in the field of consumer behavior. In this article we introduce the topic of spatial perceptions as an area of research in marketing. We extend the literature on spatial perceptions by proposing that consumers use the direct distance between the endpoints of a path, or the distance "as the crow flies," as a source of information while making distance judgments -- the shorter the direct distance, the shorter the distance estimate. We study two spatial features that affect direct distance-path angularity (i.e., the size of the angle between path segments) and path direction (i.e., whether the path retraces back or not). We further propose and demonstrate that the direct-distance bias is due to the perceptual salience of direct distance and is used by consumers in an automatic manner. Theoretical implications for the manner in which consumers process spatial information and the use of cognitive heuristics while making spatial judgments are discussed.

[1]  F. W. Finger,et al.  The illustration of the horizontal-vertical illusion. , 1947, Journal of experimental psychology.

[2]  Kevin Lynch,et al.  The Image of the City , 1960 .

[3]  John U. Farley,et al.  A Stochastic Model of Supermarket Traffic Flow , 1966, Oper. Res..

[4]  Eugene S. Edgington,et al.  An Additive Method for Combining Probability Values from Independent Experiments , 1972 .

[5]  A. Tversky,et al.  Judgment under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases , 1974, Science.

[6]  Shelley E. Taylor,et al.  Point of view and perceptions of causality. , 1975 .

[7]  D. B. MacKay,et al.  Cognitive Maps of Retail Locations: An Investigation of Some Basic Issues , 1975 .

[8]  Walter Schneider,et al.  Controlled and Automatic Human Information Processing: 1. Detection, Search, and Attention. , 1977 .

[9]  Walter Schneider,et al.  Controlled and automatic human information processing: II. Perceptual learning, automatic attending and a general theory. , 1977 .

[10]  W. Kintsch,et al.  Memory and cognition , 1977 .

[11]  G. Allen,et al.  The role of perceptual context in structuring spatial knowledge. , 1978 .

[12]  Larry Percy,et al.  Visual Imaging Ability As a Mediator of Advertising Response , 1978 .

[13]  Shelley E. Taylor,et al.  Salience, Attention, and Attribution: Top of the Head Phenomena , 1978 .

[14]  R. Rosenthal Combining results of independent studies. , 1978 .

[15]  Edward K. Sadalla,et al.  An Information Storage Model for Distance Cognition , 1980 .

[16]  P. Thorndyke Distance estimation from cognitive maps , 1981, Cognitive Psychology.

[17]  Morris B. Holbrook,et al.  Feature Interactions in Consumer Judgments of Verbal Versus Pictorial Presentations , 1981 .

[18]  G. Allen A developmental perspective on the effects of "subdividing" macrospatial experience. , 1981 .

[19]  Robert Sommer,et al.  Mental Mapping of Two Supermarkets , 1982 .

[20]  A. Tversky,et al.  Judgment under Uncertainty , 1982 .

[21]  Richard Staelin,et al.  The Information Processing of Pictures in Print Advertisements , 1983 .

[22]  Terry L. Childers,et al.  Imagery Paradigms For Consumer Research: Alternative Perspectives From Cognitive Psychology , 1983 .

[23]  Terry L. Childers,et al.  Conditions for a Picture-Superiority Effect on Consumer Memory , 1984 .

[24]  R. Ratcliff,et al.  The mental representation of knowledge acquired from maps. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[25]  Jack L. Nasar,et al.  Out of Sight Further from Mind , 1985 .

[26]  H. Wass,et al.  A Developmental Perspective , 1985 .

[27]  J. Bargh,et al.  Individual construct accessibility, person memory, and the recall-judgment link: The case of information overload. , 1985 .

[28]  T. McNamara Mental representations of spatial relations , 1986, Cognitive Psychology.

[29]  Christine A. Klein,et al.  Children's Distance Estimates in a Large-Scale Environment , 1986 .

[30]  Terry L. Childers,et al.  Picture-Word Consistency and the Elaborative Processing of Advertisements , 1987 .

[31]  Linda L. Price,et al.  The Role of Imagery in Information Processing: Review and Extensions , 1987 .

[32]  J. Antes,et al.  The Effect of a New City Traffic Route on the Cognitive Maps of Its Residents , 1988 .

[33]  J. Bargh,et al.  Context-dependent automatic processing in depression: accessibility of negative constructs with regard to self but not others. , 1988, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[34]  Automatic and dysfunctional cognitive processes in depression. , 1989 .

[35]  Daniel T. Gilbert,et al.  Thinking lightly about others: Automatic components of the social inference process. , 1989 .

[36]  J. Bargh Conditional automaticity: Varieties of automatic influence in social perception and cognition. , 1989 .

[37]  S. C. Masin,et al.  Handedness and space errors , 1991 .

[38]  Laura A. Peracchio,et al.  Getting an angle in advertising: The effect of camera angle on product evaluations. , 1992 .

[39]  Terry L. Childers,et al.  The Role of Expectancy and Relevancy in Memory for Verbal and Visual Information: What Is Incongruency? , 1992 .

[40]  P. Raghubir,et al.  The effect of line configuration on perceived numerosity of dotted lines , 1997, Memory & cognition.