Day by day, hour by hour: Naturalistic language input to infants.

Measurements of infants' quotidian experiences provide critical information about early development. However, the role of sampling methods in providing these measurements is rarely examined. Here we directly compare language input from hour-long video-recordings and daylong audio-recordings within the same group of 44 infants at 6 and 7 months. We compared 12 measures of language quantity and lexical diversity, talker variability, utterance-type, and object presence, finding moderate correlations across recording-types. However, video-recordings generally featured far denser noun input across these measures compared to the daylong audio-recordings, more akin to 'peak' audio hours (though not as high in talkers and word-types). Although audio-recordings captured ~10 times more awake-time than videos, the noun input in them was only 2-4 times greater. Notably, whether we compared videos to daylong audio-recordings or peak audio times, videos featured relatively fewer declaratives and more questions; furthermore, the most common video-recorded nouns were less consistent across families than the top audio-recording nouns were. Thus, hour-long videos and daylong audio-recordings revealed fairly divergent pictures of the language infants hear and learn from in their daily lives. We suggest that short video-recordings provide a dense and somewhat different sample of infants' language experiences, rather than a typical one, and should be used cautiously for extrapolation about common words, talkers, utterance-types, and contexts at larger timescales. If theories of language development are to be held accountable to 'facts on the ground' from observational data, greater care is needed to unpack the ramifications of sampling methods of early language input.

[1]  H. Taine Note sur l’acquisition du langage chez les enfants et dans l’espèce humaine , 1876 .

[2]  H. M. Williams An analytical study of language achievement in preschool children , 1937 .

[3]  J. Altmann,et al.  Observational study of behavior: sampling methods. , 1974, Behaviour.

[4]  S. Holm A Simple Sequentially Rejective Multiple Test Procedure , 1979 .

[5]  Gordon Wells,et al.  Characteristics of adult speech which predict children's language development , 1983, Journal of Child Language.

[6]  B. Debaryshe,et al.  Joint picture-book reading correlates of early oral language skill , 1993, Journal of Child Language.

[7]  L. Fenson,et al.  Lexical development norms for young children , 1996 .

[8]  M. Tomasello Do young children have adult syntactic competence? , 2000, Cognition.

[9]  F. Gardner Methodological Issues in the Direct Observation of Parent–Child Interaction: Do Observational Findings Reflect the Natural Behavior of Participants? , 2000, Clinical child and family psychology review.

[10]  B. MacWhinney Emergentist Approaches to Language , 2001, Encyclopedia of Evolutionary Psychological Science.

[11]  M. Brent,et al.  The role of exposure to isolated words in early vocabulary development , 2001, Cognition.

[12]  Letitia R. Naigles,et al.  How children use input to acquire a lexicon. , 2002, Child development.

[13]  J. Huttenlocher,et al.  Language input and child syntax , 2002, Cognitive Psychology.

[14]  S. Waxman,et al.  What infants know about syntax but couldn't have learned: experimental evidence for syntactic structure at 18 months , 2003, Cognition.

[15]  S. Suter Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of young American children , 2005, European Journal of Pediatrics.

[16]  M. Farah,et al.  Neurocognitive correlates of socioeconomic status in kindergarten children. , 2005, Developmental science.

[17]  Nadya Pancsofar,et al.  Mother and father language input to young children: Contributions to later language development. , 2006 .

[18]  Steven D. Levitt,et al.  What Do Laboratory Experiments Measuring Social Preferences Reveal About the Real World , 2007 .

[19]  K. Adolph,et al.  What is the shape of developmental change? , 2008, Psychological review.

[20]  Franziska Tschan,et al.  All in the Timing , 2008 .

[21]  C. Dudley-Marling,et al.  Pathologizing the Language and Culture of Poor Children. , 2009 .

[22]  D K Oller,et al.  Automated vocal analysis of naturalistic recordings from children with autism, language delay, and typical development , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[23]  Jodi A Mindell,et al.  Cross-cultural differences in infant and toddler sleep. , 2010, Sleep medicine.

[24]  Brian MacWhinney,et al.  Transcribing, searching and data sharing: The CLAN software and the TalkBank data repository. , 2010, Gesprachsforschung : Online-Zeitschrift zur verbalen Interaktion.

[25]  Bob McMurray,et al.  Finding the signal by adding noise: The role of noncontrastive phonetic variability in early word learning. , 2010, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.

[26]  Susan Goldin-Meadow,et al.  Language input and acquisition in a Mayan village: how important is directed speech? , 2012, Developmental science.

[27]  S. Michaels Commentary: Déjà Vu All Over Again: What’s Wrong With Hart & Risley and a "Linguistic Deficit" Framework in Early Childhood Education? , 2013 .

[28]  Linda B. Smith,et al.  Statistical word learning at scale: the baby's view is better. , 2013, Developmental science.

[29]  Elika Bergelson,et al.  The acquisition of abstract words by young infants , 2013, Cognition.

[30]  Susan Goldin-Meadow,et al.  Quality of early parent input predicts child vocabulary 3 years later , 2013, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[31]  Anne Fernald,et al.  SES differences in language processing skill and vocabulary are evident at 18 months. , 2013, Developmental science.

[32]  Anne Fernald,et al.  Talking to Children Matters , 2013, Psychological science.

[33]  Patrick Meehan,et al.  An Exploratory Study of “Quantitative Linguistic Feedback” , 2013 .

[34]  Michael C. Frank,et al.  Predicting the birth of a spoken word , 2015, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[35]  Christina Bergmann,et al.  Discriminability of sound contrasts in the face of speaker variation quantified , 2016, CogSci.

[36]  Alejandrina Cristia,et al.  HomeBank: An Online Repository of Daylong Child-Centered Audio Recordings , 2016, Seminars in Speech and Language.

[37]  Richard N Aslin,et al.  Nature and origins of the lexicon in 6-mo-olds , 2017, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[38]  Marc H Bornstein,et al.  Power in methods: language to infants in structured and naturalistic contexts. , 2017, Developmental science.

[39]  Michelle L. McGillion,et al.  A randomised controlled trial to test the effect of promoting caregiver contingent talk on language development in infants from diverse socioeconomic status backgrounds , 2017, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[40]  K. Scherer,et al.  The effect of context and audio-visual modality on emotions elicited by a musical performance , 2016, Psychology of music.

[41]  D. Swingley,et al.  Young Infants' Word Comprehension Given An Unfamiliar Talker or Altered Pronunciations. , 2018, Child development.

[42]  Elika Bergelson,et al.  What Do North American Babies Hear? A large-scale cross-corpus analysis. , 2018, Developmental science.

[43]  Mothers' Work Status and 17-month-olds' Productive Vocabulary. , 2018, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.

[44]  Alejandrina Cristia,et al.  Child-Directed Speech Is Infrequent in a Forager-Farmer Population: A Time Allocation Study. , 2019, Child development.