Separating deceptive and orienting components in a Concealed Information Test.

The Concealed Information Test (CIT) requires the examinee to deceptively deny recognition of known stimuli and to truthfully deny recognition of unknown stimuli. Because deception and orienting are typically coupled, it is unclear how exactly these sub-processes affect the physiological responses measured in the CIT. The present study aimed at separating the effects of deception from those of orienting. In a mock-crime study, using a modified CIT, thirty-six of seventy-two subjects answered truthfully ('truth group'), whereas the other thirty-six concealed their knowledge ('lie group'). Answering was delayed for 4 s after item presentation. Electrodermal activity (EDA), respiration (RLL), and phasic heart rate (HR) were recorded. A decomposition of EDA responses revealed two response components; the response in the first interval was expected to indicate orienting, stimulus evaluation, and answer preparation, whereas the response in the second interval was assumed to reflect answer-related processes. Inconclusively, both EDA components differentiated between 'probe' and 'irrelevant' items in both groups. Phasic HR and RLL differed between item classes only in the 'lie' group, thus reflecting answer-related processes, possibly deception, rather than merely orienting responses. The findings further support the notion that psychophysiological measures elicited by a modified CIT may reflect different mental processes involved in orienting and deception.

[1]  G. Vossel,et al.  Psychophysiological differentiation of deception: the effects of electrodermal lability and mode of responding on skin conductance and heart rate. , 2001, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[2]  Gershon Ben-Shakhar,et al.  The validity of psychophysiological detection of information with the Guilty Knowledge Test: a meta-analytic review. , 2003, The Journal of applied psychology.

[3]  Eric Seemann,et al.  Cognitive mechanisms underlying lying to questions: response time as a cue to deception , 2003 .

[4]  P. Venables,et al.  Direct measurement of skin conductance: a proposal for standardization. , 1971, Psychophysiology.

[5]  Howard W. Timm,et al.  Analyzing deception from respiration patterns. , 1982 .

[6]  J. Furedy,et al.  Electrodermal differentiation of deception: the effect of choice versus no choice of deceptive items. , 1994, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[7]  J J Furedy,et al.  The roles of deception, intention to deceive, and motivation to avoid detection in the psychophysiological detection of guilty knowledge. , 1991, Psychophysiology.

[8]  M. Gamer,et al.  Electrodermal and phasic heart rate responses in the Guilty Actions Test: comparing guilty examinees to informed and uninformed innocents. , 2008, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[9]  J J Furedy,et al.  Differentiation of deception as a psychological process: a psychophysiological approach. , 1988, Psychophysiology.

[10]  M. T. Bradley,et al.  Awareness of crime-relevant information and the Guilty Knowledge Test , 1992 .

[11]  Manfred Velden,et al.  Depicting cardiac activity over real time: A proposal for standardization. , 1987 .

[12]  M. E. Dawson Physiological detection of deception: measurement of responses to questions and answers during countermeasure maneuvers. , 1980, Psychophysiology.

[13]  C. Rennie,et al.  Dynamics of SCR, EEG, and ERP activity in an oddball paradigm with short interstimulus intervals. , 1999, Psychophysiology.

[14]  Jacob Cohen Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences , 1969, The SAGE Encyclopedia of Research Design.

[15]  G. Ben-Shakhar,et al.  Finger pulse waveform length in the detection of concealed information. , 2006, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[16]  Robert Rosenthal,et al.  Computing Contrasts, Effect Sizes, and Counternulls on Other People's Published Data: General Procedures for Research Consumers , 1996 .

[17]  I. Lieblich,et al.  The role of "lying" in psychophysiological detection. , 2008, Psychophysiology.

[18]  G. Ben-Shakhar,et al.  A further study of the dichotomization theory in detection of information. , 1977, Psychophysiology.

[19]  Electrodermal differentiation of deception: potentially confounding and influencing factors. , 1992, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[20]  M. T. Bradley,et al.  Accuracy demonstrations, threat, and the detection of deception: cardiovascular, electrodermal, and pupillary measures. , 1981, Psychophysiology.

[21]  Ewout H. Meijer,et al.  Combining skin conductance and forced choice in the detection of concealed information. , 2007, Psychophysiology.

[22]  D. C. Raskin,et al.  Effectiveness of techniques and physiological measures in the detection of deception. , 1978, Psychophysiology.

[23]  R. Barry,et al.  Preliminary process theory: towards an integrated account of the psychophysiology of cognitive processes. , 1996, Acta neurobiologiae experimentalis.

[24]  C. Rennie,et al.  Decomposing skin conductance into tonic and phasic components. , 1997, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[25]  H. Timm Effect of altered outcome expectancies stemming from placebo and feedback treatments on the validity of the guilty knowledge technique. , 1982 .

[26]  D. Bamber The area above the ordinal dominance graph and the area below the receiver operating characteristic graph , 1975 .

[27]  C. Horneman,et al.  Detectability in the card test as a function of the subject's verbal response. , 1985, Psychophysiology.

[28]  R. Barry,et al.  Heart rate deceleration is not an orienting reflex; Heart rate acceleration is not a defensive reflex , 1985, The Pavlovian journal of biological science.

[29]  Bruno Verschuere,et al.  Autonomic and behavioral responding to concealed information: differentiating orienting and defensive responses. , 2004, Psychophysiology.

[30]  G. Ben-Shakhar,et al.  Standardization within individuals: a simple method to neutralize individual differences in skin conductance. , 1985, Psychophysiology.

[31]  D. Lykken The GSR in the detection of guilt. , 1959 .

[32]  J. Kircher,et al.  The Finapres (Volume Clamp) Recording Method in Psychophysiological Detection of Deception Examinations: Experimental Comparison with the Cardiograph Method , 1999 .

[33]  Yukihiro Sawada,et al.  A new index for psychophysiological detection of deception : Applicability of normalized pulse volume , 2003 .

[34]  Jennifer M. C. Vendemia,et al.  Reaction Time of Motor Responses in Two-Stimulus Paradigms Involving Deception and Congruity with Varying Levels of Difficulty , 2005, Behavioural neurology.

[35]  Bruno Verschuere,et al.  Antisociality, underarousal and the validity of the Concealed Information Polygraph Test , 2007, Biological Psychology.

[36]  C M Seifert,et al.  Using response time measures to assess "guilty knowledge". , 2000, The Journal of applied psychology.

[37]  Electrodermal differentiation of deception: perceived accuracy and perceived memorial content manipulations. , 1991, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[38]  Matthias Gamer,et al.  Psychophysiological and vocal measures in the detection of guilty knowledge. , 2006, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[39]  Gershon Ben-Shakhar,et al.  Behavioral and physiological measures in the detection of concealed information. , 2005, The Journal of applied psychology.