The Role of Co-Occurrence Statistics in Developing Semantic Knowledge

The organization of our knowledge about the world into an interconnected network of concepts linked by relations profoundly impacts many facets of cognition, including attention, memory retrieval, reasoning, and learning. It is therefore crucial to understand how organized semantic representations are acquired. The present experiment investigated the contributions of readily observable environmental statistical regularities to semantic organization in childhood. Specifically, we investigated whether co-occurrence regularities with which entities or their labels more reliably occur together than with others (a) contribute to relations between concepts independently and (b) contribute to relations between concepts belonging to the same taxonomic category. Using child-directed speech corpora to estimate reliable co-occurrences between labels for familiar items, we constructed triads consisting of a target, a related distractor, and an unrelated distractor in which targets and related distractors consistently co-occurred (e.g., sock-foot), belonged to the same taxonomic category (e.g., sock-coat), or both (e.g., sock-shoe). We used an implicit, eye-gaze measure of relations between concepts based on the degree to which children (N = 72, age 4-7 years) looked at related versus unrelated distractors when asked to look for a target. The results indicated that co-occurrence both independently contributes to relations between concepts and contributes to relations between concepts belonging to the same taxonomic category. These findings suggest that sensitivity to the regularity with which different entities co-occur in children's environments shapes the organization of semantic knowledge during development. Implications for theoretical accounts and empirical investigations of semantic organization are discussed.

[1]  D. Mirman,et al.  Individual differences in the strength of taxonomic versus thematic relations. , 2012, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[2]  Catherine Monnier,et al.  The semantic-similarity effect in children: influence of long-term knowledge on verbal short-term memory. , 2011, The British journal of developmental psychology.

[3]  Dave M. Stampe,et al.  Heuristic filtering and reliable calibration methods for video-based pupil-tracking systems , 1993 .

[4]  Michael N Jones,et al.  Representing word meaning and order information in a composite holographic lexicon. , 2007, Psychological review.

[5]  Mark S. Seidenberg,et al.  Toddlers Activate Lexical Semantic Knowledge in the Absence of Visual Referents: Evidence from Auditory Priming. , 2013, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.

[6]  Anna V Fisher,et al.  What's in the name? Or how rocks and stones are different from bunnies and rabbits. , 2010, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[7]  Mark H. Johnson,et al.  Global-Before-Basic Object Categorization in Connectionist Networks and 2-Month-Old Infants. , 2000, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.

[8]  J. Mandler,et al.  Taxonomies and triads: Conceptual organization in one- to two-year-olds , 1989, Cognitive Psychology.

[9]  D. Bates,et al.  Fitting Linear Mixed-Effects Models Using lme4 , 2014, 1406.5823.

[10]  Michael N. Jones,et al.  Comparing Predictive and Co-occurrence Based Models of Lexical Semantics Trained on Child-directed Speech , 2016, CogSci.

[11]  E. Gordon,et al.  Defining the temporal threshold for ocular fixation in free-viewing visuocognitive tasks , 2003, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[12]  Miriam Bassok,et al.  What Makes a Man Similar to a Tie? Stimulus Compatibility with Comparison and Integration , 1999, Cognitive Psychology.

[13]  Karrie E. Godwin,et al.  The influence of label co-occurrence and semantic similarity on children’s inductive generalization , 2015, Front. Psychol..

[14]  E. Rosch,et al.  Family resemblances: Studies in the internal structure of categories , 1975, Cognitive Psychology.

[15]  L. Oakes,et al.  By land or by sea: the role of perceptual similarity in infants' categorization of animals. , 1997, Developmental psychology.

[16]  R N Aslin,et al.  Statistical Learning by 8-Month-Old Infants , 1996, Science.

[17]  Barbara Tversky,et al.  Development of taxonomic organization of named and pictured categories. , 1985 .

[18]  Katherine Nelson,et al.  Taxonomic Knowledge: What Kind and When? , 1992 .

[19]  K. Plunkett,et al.  Spoken-word recognition in 2-year-olds: The tug of war between phonological and semantic activation , 2017 .

[20]  Gregory L. Murphy,et al.  Thematic relations in adults' concepts. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[21]  Catarina Vales,et al.  When Stronger Knowledge Slows You Down: Semantic Relatedness Predicts Children's Co-Activation of Related Items in a Visual Search Paradigm , 2019, Cogn. Sci..

[22]  Ann L. Brown,et al.  Conceptual preference for thematic or taxonomic relations: A nonmonotonic age trend from preschool to old age ☆ , 1979 .

[23]  G. Bower,et al.  Hierarchical retrieval schemes in recall of categorized word lists , 1969 .

[24]  V. Sloutsky,et al.  Induction and categorization in young children: a similarity-based model. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[25]  M. Johnson,et al.  Parsing eye-tracking data of variable quality to provide accurate fixation duration estimates in infants and adults , 2012, Behavior research methods.

[26]  L. Chelazzi,et al.  Associative knowledge controls deployment of visual selective attention , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.

[27]  James L. McClelland,et al.  You shall know an object by the company it keeps: An investigation of semantic representations derived from object co-occurrence in visual scenes , 2015, Neuropsychologia.

[28]  J. Coley,et al.  Where the wild things are: informal experience and ecological reasoning. , 2012, Child development.

[29]  D. Faulkner,et al.  Perceptual, thematic and taxonomic relations in children’s mental representations: Responses to triads , 1993 .

[30]  G. Deák,et al.  The dynamics of preschoolers' categorization choices. , 1996, Child development.

[31]  P. D. Eimas,et al.  Studies on the formation of perceptually based basic-level categories in young infants. , 1994, Child development.

[32]  Scott P. Johnson,et al.  Visual statistical learning in the newborn infant , 2011, Cognition.

[33]  D. Mirman,et al.  Dynamics of activation of semantically similar concepts during spoken word recognition , 2009, Memory & cognition.

[34]  G. Altmann,et al.  Word meaning and the control of eye fixation: semantic competitor effects and the visual world paradigm , 2005, Cognition.

[35]  Vladimir M. Sloutsky,et al.  From Perceptual Categories to Concepts: What Develops? , 2010, Cogn. Sci..

[36]  T. Landauer,et al.  A Solution to Plato's Problem: The Latent Semantic Analysis Theory of Acquisition, Induction, and Representation of Knowledge. , 1997 .

[37]  Thomas T. Hills,et al.  Categorical structure among shared features in networks of early-learned nouns , 2009, Cognition.

[38]  Dorothy Tse,et al.  References and Notes Supporting Online Material Materials and Methods Figs. S1 to S5 Tables S1 to S3 Electron Impact (ei) Mass Spectra Chemical Ionization (ci) Mass Spectra References Schemas and Memory Consolidation Research Articles Research Articles Research Articles Research Articles , 2022 .

[39]  Agnès Blaye,et al.  Categorical flexibility in children: Distinguishing response flexibility from conceptual flexibility; the protracted development of taxonomic representations , 2006 .

[40]  Mirella Lapata,et al.  Incremental Bayesian Category Learning From Natural Language , 2016, Cogn. Sci..

[41]  Richard N Aslin,et al.  Nature and origins of the lexicon in 6-mo-olds , 2017, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[42]  P. Blignaut Fixation identification: The optimum threshold for a dispersion algorithm , 2009, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[43]  Y. Koike,et al.  Relatedness-dependent rapid development of brain activity in anterior temporal cortex during pair-association retrieval , 2016, Neuroscience Letters.

[44]  Jessica F. Hay,et al.  Statistical learning in a natural language by 8-month-old infants. , 2009, Child development.

[45]  Elizabeth K. Johnson,et al.  Statistical learning of tone sequences by human infants and adults , 1999, Cognition.

[46]  James L. McClelland,et al.  The parallel distributed processing approach to semantic cognition , 2003, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[47]  S. P. Nguyen,et al.  Cross-classification and category representation in children's concepts. , 2007, Developmental psychology.

[48]  Philip A. Huebner,et al.  Structured Semantic Knowledge Can Emerge Automatically from Predicting Word Sequences in Child-Directed Speech , 2018, Front. Psychol..

[49]  D. Medin,et al.  Categorization and Reasoning among Tree Experts: Do All Roads Lead to Rome? , 1997, Cognitive Psychology.

[50]  D. Spence,et al.  Lexical co-occurrence and association strength , 1990 .

[51]  Anna V. Fisher,et al.  Processing of perceptual information is more robust than processing of conceptual information in preschool-age children: Evidence from costs of switching , 2011, Cognition.

[52]  Michelene T. H. Chi,et al.  How knowledge is structured and used by expert and novice children , 1986 .

[53]  Kim Plunkett,et al.  How do infants build a semantic system? , 2009, Language and Cognition.

[54]  E. Heit Properties of inductive reasoning , 2000, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[55]  Rebecca Nugent,et al.  Developmental changes in semantic knowledge organization. , 2016, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[56]  Christian Biemann,et al.  Simple Co-Occurrence Statistics Reproducibly Predict Association Ratings , 2018, Cogn. Sci..

[57]  Brian H. Ross,et al.  Food for Thought: Cross-Classification and Category Organization in a Complex Real-World Domain , 1999, Cognitive Psychology.

[58]  Mutsumi Imai,et al.  Children's Theories of Word Meaning: The Role of Shape Similarity in Early Acquisition , 1994 .

[59]  Erica H Wojcik,et al.  Toddlers encode similarities among novel words from meaningful sentences , 2015, Cognition.

[60]  P. D. Eimas,et al.  Evidence for Representations of Perceptually Similar Natural Categories by 3-Month-Old and 4-Month-Old Infants , 1993, Perception.

[61]  K. Nelson,et al.  The syntagmatic-paradigmatic shift revisited: a review of research and theory. , 1977, Psychological bulletin.

[62]  Karrie E. Godwin,et al.  Semantic similarity of labels and inductive generalization: Taking a second look , 2011, Cognition.

[63]  Sharon L. Thompson-Schill,et al.  Looking for meaning: Eye movements are sensitive to overlapping semantic features, not association , 2009, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[64]  Heidi Kloos,et al.  PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Article When Looks Are Everything Appearance Similarity Versus Kind Information in Early Induction , 2022 .