Modulation of Conflict Processing by Theta-Range tACS over the Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex

Behavioral response conflict arises in the color-word Stroop task and triggers the cognitive control network. Midfrontal theta-band oscillations correlate with adaptive control mechanisms during and after conflict resolution. In order to prove causality, in two experiments, we applied transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS) at 6 Hz to the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) during Stroop task performance. Sham stimulation served as a control in both experiments; 9.7 Hz tACS served as a nonharmonic alpha band control in the second experiment. We employed generalized linear mixed models for analysis of behavioral data. Accuracy remained unchanged by any type of active stimulation. Over both experiments, the Stroop effect (response time difference between congruent and incongruent trials) was reduced by 6 Hz stimulation as compared to sham, mainly in trials without prior conflict adaptation. Alpha tACS did not modify the Stroop effect. Theta tACS can both reduce the Stroop effect and modulate adaptive mechanisms of the cognitive control network, suggesting midfrontal theta oscillations as causally involved in cognitive control.

[1]  A. Antal,et al.  Anodal tDCS Over the Left DLPFC Did Not Affect the Encoding and Retrieval of Verbal Declarative Information , 2017, Front. Neurosci..

[2]  John A. Nelder,et al.  A Simplex Method for Function Minimization , 1965, Comput. J..

[3]  M. Botvinick,et al.  Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. , 2001, Psychological review.

[4]  Walter Schneider,et al.  The cognitive control network: Integrated cortical regions with dissociable functions , 2007, NeuroImage.

[5]  M. Botvinick,et al.  Anterior cingulate cortex, error detection, and the online monitoring of performance. , 1998, Science.

[6]  K. Hornik,et al.  A Lego System for Conditional Inference , 2006 .

[7]  Abhishek Datta,et al.  Physiological and modeling evidence for focal transcranial electrical brain stimulation in humans: A basis for high-definition tDCS , 2013, NeuroImage.

[8]  N. Cohen,et al.  Prefrontal regions play a predominant role in imposing an attentional 'set': evidence from fMRI. , 2000, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[9]  Rolf Ulrich,et al.  Automatic and controlled stimulus processing in conflict tasks: Superimposed diffusion processes and delta functions , 2015, Cognitive Psychology.

[10]  M. Nitsche,et al.  Effects of Transcranial Electrical Stimulation on Cognition , 2012, Clinical EEG and neuroscience.

[11]  J. Hart,et al.  Exploring the effects of anodal and cathodal high definition transcranial direct current stimulation targeting the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex , 2018, Scientific Reports.

[12]  C. Eriksen,et al.  Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task , 1974 .

[13]  Michael X. Cohen,et al.  Midfrontal conflict-related theta-band power reflects neural oscillations that predict behavior. , 2013, Journal of neurophysiology.

[14]  Shinichiro Koga,et al.  Electrocorticographic correlates of cognitive control in a stroop task—intracranial recording in epileptic patients , 2011, Human brain mapping.

[15]  Y. Samson,et al.  Frontal dysfunction in neurologically normal chronic alcoholic subjects: metabolic and neuropsychological findings , 1998, Psychological Medicine.

[16]  D. Bates,et al.  Parsimonious Mixed Models , 2015, 1506.04967.

[17]  M. Pallak,et al.  The effect of arousal on Stroop color-word task performance , 1975 .

[18]  Abhishek Datta,et al.  Technique and Considerations in the Use of 4x1 Ring High-definition Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation (HD-tDCS) , 2013, Journal of visualized experiments : JoVE.

[19]  H. Mayberg,et al.  An ERP study of the temporal course of the Stroop color-word interference effect , 2000, Neuropsychologia.

[20]  Charles E. McCulloch Chapter 4: Generalized linear mixed models (GLMMs) , 2003 .

[21]  R. Compton,et al.  Alpha power is influenced by performance errors. , 2009, Psychophysiology.

[22]  Gregory A. Miller,et al.  The time course of activity in dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and anterior cingulate cortex during top-down attentional control , 2010, NeuroImage.

[23]  Axel Thielscher,et al.  Field modeling for transcranial magnetic stimulation: A useful tool to understand the physiological effects of TMS? , 2015, 2015 37th Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society (EMBC).

[24]  C. Carter,et al.  Anterior cingulate cortex and conflict detection: An update of theory and data , 2007, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[25]  H. Soininen,et al.  Decline of Frontal Lobe Functions in Subjects with Age-associated Memory Impairment , 1997, Neurology.

[26]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Conflict monitoring and anterior cingulate cortex: an update , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[27]  Jonathan W. Peirce,et al.  PsychoPy—Psychophysics software in Python , 2007, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[28]  G. Thut,et al.  Inconsistent Effects of Parietal α-tACS on Pseudoneglect across Two Experiments: A Failed Internal Replication , 2017, Front. Psychol..

[29]  A. Engel,et al.  Entrainment of Brain Oscillations by Transcranial Alternating Current Stimulation , 2014, Current Biology.

[30]  C Alain,et al.  Event-related neural activity associated with the Stroop task. , 1999, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[31]  Colin M. Macleod The Stroop Task in Cognitive Research. , 2005 .

[32]  Per B. Brockhoff,et al.  lmerTest Package: Tests in Linear Mixed Effects Models , 2017 .

[33]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Anterior Cingulate Conflict Monitoring and Adjustments in Control , 2004, Science.

[34]  Antao Chen,et al.  The neural oscillations of conflict adaptation in the human frontal region , 2013, Biological Psychology.

[35]  A. Chang,et al.  One night of sleep deprivation affects reaction time, but not interference or facilitation in a Stroop task , 2011, Brain and Cognition.

[36]  A. Diamond Executive functions. , 2014, Handbook of clinical neurology.

[37]  N. Breslow,et al.  Approximate inference in generalized linear mixed models , 1993 .

[38]  Nicholas Wymbs,et al.  Neural correlates of conflict processing , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.

[39]  D. Bates,et al.  Fitting Linear Mixed-Effects Models Using lme4 , 2014, 1406.5823.

[40]  Mollie E. Brooks,et al.  Generalized linear mixed models: a practical guide for ecology and evolution. , 2009, Trends in ecology & evolution.

[41]  Axel Thielscher,et al.  On the importance of electrode parameters for shaping electric field patterns generated by tDCS , 2015, NeuroImage.

[42]  E. Donchin,et al.  Optimizing the use of information: strategic control of activation of responses. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[43]  Roger Ratcliff,et al.  A Theory of Memory Retrieval. , 1978 .

[44]  F. Fröhlich,et al.  Transcranial Alternating Current Stimulation Modulates Large-Scale Cortical Network Activity by Network Resonance , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[45]  Simon Hanslmayr,et al.  The Electrophysiological Dynamics of Interference during the Stroop Task , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[46]  J. R. Simon,et al.  Auditory S-R compatibility: the effect of an irrelevant cue on information processing. , 1967, The Journal of applied psychology.

[47]  A. Pahor,et al.  The effects of theta transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS) on fluid intelligence. , 2014, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[48]  Simon Hanslmayr,et al.  Neural Communication Patterns Underlying Conflict Detection, Resolution, and Adaptation , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[49]  M. Lavidor,et al.  Transcranial Alternating Current Stimulation Increases Risk-Taking Behavior in the Balloon Analog Risk Task , 2011, Front. Neurosci..

[50]  C. Pantelis,et al.  A brief assessment of executive control dysfunction: discriminant validity and homogeneity of planning, set shift, and fluency measures. , 1996, Archives of clinical neuropsychology : the official journal of the National Academy of Neuropsychologists.

[51]  Walter Paulus,et al.  Spatial Working Memory in Humans Depends on Theta and High Gamma Synchronization in the Prefrontal Cortex , 2016, Current Biology.

[52]  S. Yamaguchi,et al.  Theta band transcranial alternating current stimulations modulates network behavior of dorsal anterior cingulate cortex , 2017, Scientific Reports.

[53]  Kurt Hornik,et al.  Implementing a Class of Permutation Tests: The coin Package , 2008 .

[54]  G. Logan,et al.  Testing the validity of conflict drift-diffusion models for use in estimating cognitive processes: A parameter-recovery study , 2018, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[55]  Sally Andrews,et al.  To transform or not to transform: using generalized linear mixed models to analyse reaction time data , 2015, Front. Psychol..

[56]  M. Frank,et al.  Frontal theta as a mechanism for cognitive control , 2014, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[57]  A. Antal,et al.  Comparatively weak after-effects of transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS) on cortical excitability in humans , 2008, Brain Stimulation.

[58]  O. Spreen,et al.  A Compendium of Neuropsychological Tests: Administration, Norms, and Commentary , 1991 .

[59]  M. Brass,et al.  Involvement of the inferior frontal junction in cognitive control: Meta‐analyses of switching and Stroop studies , 2005, Human brain mapping.

[60]  J. Stroop Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. , 1992 .

[61]  Michael X. Cohen,et al.  Frequency Band-Specific Electrical Brain Stimulation Modulates Cognitive Control Processes , 2015, PloS one.