Artificial Grammar Learning Capabilities in an Abstract Visual Task Match Requirements for Linguistic Syntax
暂无分享,去创建一个
W. Tecumseh Fitch | Carlo Cecchetto | Gesche Westphal-Fitch | Beatrice Giustolisi | Jordan S. Martin | W. Fitch | C. Cecchetto | J. S. Martin | Gesche Westphal-Fitch | B. Giustolisi
[1] E. Luby. On the nature of memory. , 1966, Michigan medicine.
[2] R N Aslin,et al. Statistical Learning by 8-Month-Old Infants , 1996, Science.
[3] Peter M. Vishton,et al. Rule learning by seven-month-old infants. , 1999, Science.
[4] Sophia Rabe-Hesketh,et al. Redundant Overdispersion Parameters in Multilevel Models for Categorical Responses , 2007 .
[5] A. Gelman. Prior distributions for variance parameters in hierarchical models (comment on article by Browne and Draper) , 2004 .
[6] Kazuo Okanoya,et al. Revisiting the syntactic abilities of non-human animals: natural vocalizations and artificial grammar learning , 2012, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[7] Timothy Q. Gentner,et al. Recursive syntactic pattern learning by songbirds , 2006, Nature.
[8] Stuart M. Shieber,et al. Evidence against the context-freeness of natural language , 1985 .
[9] Elissa L. Newport,et al. The role of constituent structure in the induction of an artificial language , 1981 .
[10] P. Diggle. Analysis of Longitudinal Data , 1995 .
[11] W. Tecumseh Fitch,et al. Action at a distance: dependency sensitivity in a New World primate , 2013, Biology Letters.
[12] W Tecumseh Fitch,et al. Toward a computational framework for cognitive biology: unifying approaches from cognitive neuroscience and comparative cognition. , 2014, Physics of life reviews.
[13] Alaa A. Kharbouch,et al. Three models for the description of language , 1956, IRE Trans. Inf. Theory.
[14] Matthias Sebastian Treder,et al. Behind the Looking-Glass: A Review on Human Symmetry Perception , 2010, Symmetry.
[15] W. Tecumseh Fitch,et al. Visual artificial grammar learning: comparative research on humans, kea (Nestor notabilis) and pigeons (Columba livia) , 2012, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[16] Aravind K. Joshi,et al. Processing crossed and nested dependencies: An automation perspective on the psycholinguistic results , 1990 .
[17] Willem H. Zuidema,et al. Simple rules can explain discrimination of putative recursive syntactic structures by a songbird species , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[18] Peter Hagoort,et al. Implicit Acquisition of Grammars With Crossed and Nested Non-Adjacent Dependencies: Investigating the Push-Down Stack Model , 2012, Cogn. Sci..
[19] Richard B. D’Eath,et al. Can video images imitate real stimuli in animal behaviour experiments? , 1998 .
[20] Morten H. Christiansen,et al. Processing multiple non-adjacent dependencies: evidence from sequence learning , 2012, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[21] William A. Link,et al. On thinning of chains in MCMC , 2012 .
[22] Edward P. Stabler,et al. Varieties of crossing dependencies: structure dependence and mild context sensitivity , 2004, Cogn. Sci..
[23] J. Saffran,et al. Dog is a dog is a dog: Infant rule learning is not specific to language , 2007, Cognition.
[24] W. Fitch,et al. Non-adjacent visual dependency learning in chimpanzees , 2015, Animal Cognition.
[25] Esther Mondragón,et al. Rule Learning by Rats , 2008, Science.
[26] A. Anwander,et al. The brain differentiates human and non-human grammars: Functional localization and structural connectivity , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[27] A. Reber. Implicit learning of artificial grammars , 1967 .
[28] S. Dehaene,et al. Cortical representation of the constituent structure of sentences , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[29] Todd M. Bailey,et al. The role of similarity in artificial grammar learning. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[30] Carlo Geraci,et al. How grammar can cope with limited short-term memory: Simultaneity and seriality in sign languages , 2008, Cognition.
[31] W. Fitch,et al. More than one way to see it: Individual heuristics in avian visual computation , 2015, Cognition.
[32] Morten H. Christiansen,et al. Domain generality versus modality specificity: the paradox of statistical learning , 2015, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[33] F. Pulvermüller. Brain embodiment of syntax and grammar: Discrete combinatorial mechanisms spelt out in neuronal circuits , 2010, Brain and Language.
[34] Angela D. Friederici,et al. Artificial grammar learning meets formal language theory: an overview , 2012, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[35] L R Squire,et al. Intact learning of artificial grammars and intact category learning by patients with Parkinson's disease. , 1999, Behavioral neuroscience.
[36] Carel ten Cate,et al. Artificial grammar learning in zebra finches and human adults: XYX versus XXY , 2014, Animal Cognition.
[37] P. Hagoort. On Broca, brain, and binding: a new framework , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[38] M. Wand,et al. General design Bayesian generalized linear mixed models , 2006, math/0606491.
[39] William D. Marslen-Wilson,et al. Crossed and nested dependencies in German and Dutch , 1986 .
[40] Jarrod Had. MCMC Methods for Multi-Response Generalized Linear Mixed Models: The MCMCglmm R Package , 2010 .
[41] Yukiko Kikuchi,et al. Auditory Artificial Grammar Learning in Macaque and Marmoset Monkeys , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[42] Andrew Gelman,et al. Why We (Usually) Don't Have to Worry About Multiple Comparisons , 2009, 0907.2478.
[43] Birgit Öttl,et al. Does Formal Complexity Reflect Cognitive Complexity? Investigating Aspects of the Chomsky Hierarchy in an Artificial Language Learning Study , 2015, PloS one.
[44] Willem H. Zuidema. Context-freeness Revisited , 2013, CogSci.
[45] P. Niyogi,et al. Computational and evolutionary aspects of language , 2002, Nature.
[46] Johan Wagemans,et al. Characteristics and models of human symmetry detection , 1997, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[47] Elizabeth K. Johnson,et al. Statistical learning of tone sequences by human infants and adults , 1999, Cognition.
[48] W. Fitch,et al. Computational Constraints on Syntactic Processing in a Nonhuman Primate , 2004, Science.
[49] Karsten Steinhauer,et al. Brain signatures of artificial language processing: Evidence challenging the critical period hypothesis , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[50] James Rogers,et al. Formal language theory: refining the Chomsky hierarchy , 2012, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[51] L. Squire,et al. Artificial grammar learning depends on implicit acquisition of both abstract and exemplar-specific information. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[52] Zoltan Dienes,et al. The nature of the memory buffer in implicit learning: Learning Chinese tonal symmetries , 2013, Consciousness and Cognition.
[53] Shinichi Nakagawa,et al. Repeatability for Gaussian and non‐Gaussian data: a practical guide for biologists , 2010, Biological reviews of the Cambridge Philosophical Society.
[54] William J. Idsardi,et al. What Complexity Differences Reveal About Domains in Language , 2013, Top. Cogn. Sci..
[55] Jeffrey Heinz,et al. Sentence and Word Complexity , 2011, Science.