Human Supervisory Performance in Flexible Manufacturing Systems

The Flexible Manufacturing System (FMS) integrates humans, computers, robots, and computerized machines in order to meet system goals. The objective of this paper is to explore both the optimal allocation of tasks between human supervisor and computers and the optimal number of machines which a supervisor should control in an FMS. The hypotheses are proposed that there is an interaction effect between the number of machines and the task allocation level and that there exists an optimal level of number of machines for the operator to monitor. These hypotheses were tested in a statistically balanced experimental design using 30 subjects with three independent variables: number of machines (5 levels–4, 8, 12, 16, or 20 machines), task allocation (2 levels–high or low), and sequence of task presentation (2 levels). Physiological measures (EKG and respiratory rate) and two subjective stress questionnaires were used as indices of the arousal level. The results reveal that the Inverted-U function for response time as a function of machine level was not supported and the allocation level was significant for both performance and subjective stress criteria. These results suggest that in the supervision of FMS the task allocation to the supervisor is critical but that the size of the FMS system (4 to 20 machines), with the allocations studied, has no effect on either stress or performance of the operator in the FMS.

[1]  E Fantino,et al.  The Yerkes-Dodson law and alimentary motivation. , 1970, Canadian journal of psychology.

[2]  R. Yerkes,et al.  The relation of strength of stimulus to rapidity of habit‐formation , 1908 .

[3]  Sheue-Ling Hwang,et al.  Management Strategies for the Design, Control and Operation of Flexible Manufacturing Systems , 1983 .

[4]  R. G. Pearson,et al.  Scale analysis of a fatigue checklist. , 1957 .

[5]  G. Salvendy,et al.  External and internal attentional environments. II. Reconsideration of the relationship between sinus arrhythmia and information load. , 1982, Ergonomics.

[6]  Mark Hochhauser,et al.  Cue Effects of Drive and Reward as a Function of Discrimination Difficulty: Evidence Against the Yerkes-Dodson Law. , 1975 .

[7]  J. R. Royce,et al.  A multifactor-system dynamics theory of emotion , 1980 .

[8]  Woodrow Barfield,et al.  Stress as a Function of the Rate at Which Information is Presented on a Video Display Terminal* , 1983 .

[9]  Willy Lens,et al.  Achievement-motivation and intelligence-test scores - a test of the yerkes-dodson hypothesis , 1980 .

[10]  L. Pinneo,et al.  The effects of induced muscle tension during tracking on level of activation and on performance. , 1961, Journal of experimental psychology.

[11]  R. D. Edsell Noise and Social Interaction as Simultaneous Stressors , 1976 .

[12]  J. Hokanson,et al.  EFFECTS OF PHYSIOLOGICAL AROUSAL LEVEL, FRUSTRATION, AND TASK COMPLEXITY ON PERFORMANCE. , 1964, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[13]  W. Wierwille,et al.  A Comparison of Rating Scale, Secondary-Task, Physiological, and Primary-Task Workload Estimation Techniques in a Simulated Flight Task Emphasizing Communications Load , 1983, Human factors.