Do adults produce phonetic variants of /t/ less often in speech to children?

Abstract The surface phonetic details of an utterance affect how ‘native’ a speaker sounds. However, studies have shown that children’s acquisition of context-appropriate variation (sometimes called allophones) is late. This study’s goal was to understand how caregivers use phonetic variation in the production of American English /t/ in child-directed speech (CDS), compared to in adult-directed speech (ADS). We hypothesized that mothers modify their input to children in order to produce more limited variation in CDS than in ADS, to potentially assist children in the development of contrastive phonemic categories. To this end, we recorded eight mothers of children under the age of 2 years in both ADS and CDS conditions. Results reveal that CDS contains significantly more canonical cues to /t/ than ADS does, and fewer non-canonical cue patterns, including fewer unreleased tokens and fewer glottalized tokens in utterance-medial position. Also, we found larger aspiration duration differences in CDS between aspirated singleton [th] vs. unaspirated [t] in /st/ contexts, suggesting that mothers exaggerate this cue to the phonemic context in which the /t/ occurs. Overall, the findings suggest that CDS more clearly signals the phonemic category, which could in turn assist children learn the relationship between the underlying and surface forms.

[1]  S. Kemper,et al.  Different Effects of Dual Task Demands on the Speech of Young and Older Adults , 2005, Neuropsychology, development, and cognition. Section B, Aging, neuropsychology and cognition.

[2]  V. Zue,et al.  Acoustic study of medial /t,d/ in American English , 1979 .

[3]  D. Ingram Phonological Disability in Children , 1976 .

[4]  P. Broen,et al.  The Verbal Environment of the Language-Learning Child. ASHA Monographs, No. 17. , 1972 .

[5]  D. Bonett,et al.  Sample size requirements for estimating pearson, kendall and spearman correlations , 2000 .

[6]  Kenneth de Jong,et al.  Stress-related variation in the articulation of coda alveolar stops: flapping revisited , 1998 .

[7]  N. Bernstein-Ratner Dissociations between vowel durations and formant frequency characteristics. , 1985, Journal of speech and hearing research.

[8]  Björn Lindblom,et al.  Explaining Phonetic Variation: A Sketch of the H&H Theory , 1990 .

[9]  André Malécot,et al.  The /t/ : /d/ distinction in American alveolar flai's , 1968 .

[10]  Derek M. Houston,et al.  Statistical distributions of consonant variants in infant-directed speech: Evidence that /t/ may be exceptional , 2019, J. Phonetics.

[11]  Kjellrun T. Englund Voice onset time in infant directed speech over the first six months , 2005 .

[12]  Nivja H. Jong,et al.  Praat script to detect syllable nuclei and measure speech rate automatically , 2009, Behavior research methods.

[13]  Laura C. Dilley,et al.  Phonetic variation in consonants in infant-directed and adult-directed speech: the case of regressive place assimilation in word-final alveolar stops* , 2013, Journal of Child Language.

[14]  A. Fernald Intonation and Communicative Intent in Mothers' Speech to Infants: Is the Melody the Message?. , 1989 .

[15]  H. Klein,et al.  The acquisition of medial /t, d/ allophones in bisyllabic contexts , 2002, Clinical linguistics & phonetics.

[16]  A. Seidl,et al.  The hyperarticulation hypothesis of infant-directed speech* , 2013, Journal of Child Language.

[17]  Allard Jongman,et al.  An acoustic and perceptual analysis of /t/ and /d/ flaps in American English , 2010, J. Phonetics.

[18]  D. Burnham,et al.  What's New, Pussycat? On Talking to Babies and Animals , 2002, Science.

[19]  Elena Lieven,et al.  Maternal speech to infants at 1 and 3 months of age , 2005 .

[20]  P. Keating,et al.  Articulatory strengthening at edges of prosodic domains. , 1997, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[21]  D. Bates,et al.  Fitting Linear Mixed-Effects Models Using lme4 , 2014, 1406.5823.

[22]  A. Murray,et al.  The Language Environment of Toddlers in Center-based Care versus Home Settings , 2006 .

[23]  H. Schaffer,et al.  Maternal Control Techniques in a Directed Play Situation , 1979 .

[24]  R. Newman,et al.  Infant-directed speech (IDS) vowel clarity and child language outcomes* , 2016, Journal of Child Language.

[25]  A. Fernald,et al.  A cross-language study of prosodic modifications in mothers' and fathers' speech to preverbal infants , 1989, Journal of Child Language.

[26]  D. Burnham,et al.  Universality and specificity in infant-directed speech: Pitch modifications as a function of infant age and sex in a tonal and non-tonal language , 2001 .

[27]  Stefanie Shattuck-Hufnagel,et al.  Development of phonetic variants (allophones) in 2-year-olds learning American English: A study of alveolar stop /t, d/ codas , 2015, J. Phonetics.

[28]  Nobuo Masataka,et al.  Motherese in a signed language , 1992 .

[29]  Sahyang Kim,et al.  Prosodic strengthening on the /s/-stop cluster and the phonetic implementation of an allophonic rule in English , 2014, J. Phonetics.

[30]  P. Kuhl,et al.  Cross-language analysis of phonetic units in language addressed to infants. , 1997, Science.

[31]  D. K. Oller,et al.  The effect of position in utterance on speech segment duration in English. , 1973, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[32]  Caroline Jones,et al.  Phonological reduction in maternal speech in northern Australian English: change over time* , 2013, Journal of Child Language.

[33]  A. Lowit,et al.  Rhythmic performance in hypokinetic dysarthria: Relationship between reading, spontaneous speech and diadochokinetic tasks , 2018, Journal of communication disorders.

[34]  Tamás Gábor Csapó,et al.  Word-Initial Irregular Phonation as a Function of Speech Rate and Vowel Quality in Hungarian , 2017, ISSP.

[35]  L. Shockey,et al.  Phonological Processes in Speech Addressed to Children , 1980 .

[36]  Anne Fernald,et al.  Prosody and focus in speech to infants and adults , 1991 .

[37]  D. Klatt Linguistic uses of segmental duration in English: acoustic and perceptual evidence. , 1976, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[38]  Benjamin V. Tucker The effect of reduction on the processing of flaps and /g/ in isolated words , 2011, J. Phonetics.

[39]  A. Fernald,et al.  Expanded Intonation Contours in Mothers' Speech to Newborns. , 1984 .

[40]  N. Ratner Patterns of vowel modification in mother–child speech , 1984, Journal of Child Language.

[41]  Dani Byrd,et al.  Relations of sex and dialect to reduction , 1994, Speech Communication.

[42]  R Core Team,et al.  R: A language and environment for statistical computing. , 2014 .

[43]  Nan Bernstein Ratner,et al.  Cues to post-vocalic voicing in mother-child speech , 1984 .

[44]  P. Foulkes,et al.  Phonological Variation in Child-Directed Speech , 2005 .

[45]  Susan Kemper,et al.  The costs of doing two things at once for young and older adults: talking while walking, finger tapping, and ignoring speech or noise. , 2003, Psychology and aging.

[46]  Maria Uther,et al.  Do you speak E-NG-L-I-SH? A comparison of foreigner- and infant-directed speech , 2007, Speech Commun..

[47]  Nan Bernstein Ratner,et al.  Phonological rule usage in mother-child speech , 1984 .

[48]  Mietta Lennes,et al.  Speech rate and pauses in non-native Finnish , 2009, INTERSPEECH.