The Magnitude of the Fixation Offset Effect with Endogenously and Exogenously Controlled Saccades

Two experiments examined saccadic reaction time (RT) in response to visual targets as a function of fixation offset condition (no offset; target simultaneous with offset and 200-msec offset-target SOA) in prosaccade and antisaccade tasks. The second experiment also included a condition in which saccades were made in response to verbal commands presented auditorally. To ensure that observers were equally prepared in each condition, auditory warning tones preceded target onset on every trial. The RT reduction associated with fixation offset (FOE, or gap effect) was identical with visual targets in the prosaccade task and in response to verbal signals, strongly implicating motor, rather than sensory, mechanisms in the FOE. The FOE in the antisaccade task was significant, but it was also significantly smaller than in the other tasks. We speculate that the reduced FOE in the antisaccade task may be due to the requirement to inhibit the superior colliculus when the target directed saccadic programs are, by instruction, erroneous.

[1]  C. Bruce,et al.  Primate frontal eye fields. I. Single neurons discharging before saccades. , 1985, Journal of neurophysiology.

[2]  J. L. Conway,et al.  Deficits in eye movements following frontal eye-field and superior colliculus ablations. , 1980, Journal of neurophysiology.

[3]  M. Schlag-Rey,et al.  How the frontal eye field can impose a saccade goal on superior colliculus neurons. , 1992, Journal of neurophysiology.

[4]  D. Munoz,et al.  A neural correlate for the gap effect on saccadic reaction times in monkey. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[5]  R. Wurtz,et al.  Fixation cells in monkey superior colliculus. II. Reversible activation and deactivation. , 1993, Journal of neurophysiology.

[6]  John H. R. Maunsell,et al.  The effect of frontal eye field and superior colliculus lesions on saccadic latencies in the rhesus monkey. , 1987, Journal of neurophysiology.

[7]  Alan Kingstone,et al.  Orienting of Visual Attention , 1992 .

[8]  Alan Kingstone,et al.  Why do visual offsets reduce saccadic latencies? , 1993, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[9]  P. Reuter-Lorenz,et al.  The reduction of saccadic latency by prior offset of the fixation point: An analysis of the gap effect , 1991, Perception & psychophysics.

[10]  P. Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Visuospatial coding in primate prefrontal neurons revealed by oculomotor paradigms. , 1990, Journal of neurophysiology.

[11]  R. Wurtz,et al.  Activity of superior colliculus in behaving monkey. I. Visual receptive fields of single neurons. , 1972, Journal of neurophysiology.

[12]  M. Schlag-Rey,et al.  Evidence for a supplementary eye field. , 1987, Journal of neurophysiology.

[13]  M. Saslow Effects of components of displacement-step stimuli upon latency for saccadic eye movement. , 1967, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[14]  R. Wurtz,et al.  Role of the rostral superior colliculus in active visual fixation and execution of express saccades. , 1992, Journal of neurophysiology.

[15]  Y Agid,et al.  Cortical control of reflexive visually-guided saccades. , 1991, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[16]  R A Abrams,et al.  Fixation point offsets facilitate endogenous saccades , 1998, Perception & psychophysics.

[17]  R. Klein,et al.  What are human express saccades? , 1993, Perception & psychophysics.

[18]  M. Goldberg,et al.  Functional properties of corticotectal neurons in the monkey's frontal eye field. , 1987, Journal of neurophysiology.

[19]  K. Kopecz,et al.  Saccadic reaction times in gap/overlap paradigms: a model based on integration of intentional and visual information on neural, dynamic fields , 1995, Vision Research.

[20]  B. Fischer,et al.  Saccadic eye movements after extremely short reaction times in the monkey , 1983, Brain Research.