Illusory motion perception in blindsight

Motion detection is typically spared in blindsight, which results from damage to the striate cortex (area V1) of the brain that is sufficient to eliminate conscious visual awareness and severely reduce sensitivity to luminance contrast, especially for high spatial and low temporal frequencies. Here we show that the discrimination of motion direction within cortically blind fields is not attributable to feature tracking (the detection of changes in position or shape), but is due instead to the detection of first-order motion energy (spatiotemporal changes in luminance). The key to this finding was a version of the line motion illusion entailing reverse-phi motion in which opposing motion directions are simultaneously cued by motion energy and changes in stimulus shape. In forced-choice tests, a blindsighted test subject selected the direction cued by shape change when the stimulus was presented in his intact field, but reliably selected the direction cued by motion energy when the same stimulus was presented in his blind field, where relevant position information was either inaccessible or invalid. Motion energy has been characterized as objectless, so reliance on motion energy detection is consistent with impaired access to shape information in blindsight. The dissociation of motion direction by visual field (cortically blind vs. intact) provides evidence that two pathways from the retina to MT/V5 (the cortical area specialized for motion perception) are functionally distinct: the retinogeniculate pathway through V1 is specialized for feature-based motion perception, whereas the retinocollicular pathway, which bypasses V1, is specialized for detecting motion energy.

[1]  O. Hikosaka,et al.  Focal visual attention produces illusory temporal order and motion sensation , 1993, Vision Research.

[2]  David A. Leopold,et al.  Blindsight depends on the lateral geniculate nucleus , 2010, Nature.

[3]  K H Ruddock,et al.  Residual vision in patients with retrogeniculate lesions of the visual pathways. , 1987, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[4]  A. Cowey,et al.  Pattern electroretinograms after cerebral hemispherectomy. , 2001, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[5]  Alan Cowey,et al.  The neurobiology of blindsight , 1991, Trends in Neurosciences.

[6]  George Sperling,et al.  A Systems Analysis of Visual Motion Perception , 1999 .

[7]  A. Cowey,et al.  Motion discrimination in cortically blind patients. , 2001, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[8]  G. Riddoch DISSOCIATION OF VISUAL PERCEPTIONS DUE TO OCCIPITAL INJURIES, WITH ESPECIAL REFERENCE TO APPRECIATION OF MOVEMENT , 1917 .

[9]  Takeo Watanabe,et al.  High-Level Motion Processing , 1998 .

[10]  Basileios Kroustallis,et al.  Blindsight , 2007, Scholarpedia.

[11]  P Cavanagh,et al.  Attention-based motion perception. , 1992, Science.

[12]  T. Albright,et al.  Motion mechanisms in macaque MT. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[13]  C. Gross Contribution of striate cortex and the superior colliculus to visual function in area MT, the superior temporal polysensory area and inferior temporal cortex , 1991, Neuropsychologia.

[14]  R H Wurtz,et al.  Role of striate cortex and superior colliculus in visual guidance of saccadic eye movements in monkeys. , 1977, Journal of neurophysiology.

[15]  M. J. Morgan,et al.  Motion displacement thresholds for compound stimuli predicted by the displacement of centroids , 1994, Vision Research.

[16]  G. Holmes DISTURBANCES OF VISION BY CEREBRAL LESIONS , 1918, The British journal of ophthalmology.

[17]  A. Morland,et al.  Visual perception of motion, luminance and colour in a human hemianope. , 1999, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[18]  L. Weiskrantz,et al.  Spatial and temporal response properties of residual vision in a case of hemianopia. , 1994, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[19]  Paul Azzopardi,et al.  Blindsight and Visual Awareness , 1998, Consciousness and Cognition.

[20]  Howard S. Hock,et al.  Separating the edge-based detection of object motion from the detection of objectless motion energy: implications for visually guided locomotion , 2005, IS&T/SPIE Electronic Imaging.

[21]  N. Logothetis,et al.  Role of the color-opponent and broad-band channels in vision , 1990, Visual Neuroscience.

[22]  W. Newsome,et al.  Motion selectivity in macaque visual cortex. III. Psychophysics and physiology of apparent motion. , 1986, Journal of neurophysiology.

[23]  J. Sharpe,et al.  Motion direction discrimination in blind hemifields , 1997, Annals of neurology.

[24]  H. Wilson,et al.  A psychophysically motivated model for two-dimensional motion perception , 1992, Visual Neuroscience.

[25]  R F Hess,et al.  Spatial and temporal contrast sensitivity in hemianopia. A comparative study of the sighted and blind hemifields. , 1989, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[26]  M. J. Morgan,et al.  Spatial filtering precedes motion detection , 1992, Nature.

[27]  Robert W. Kentridge,et al.  Shape Processing Area LO and Illusory Contours , 2009, Perception.

[28]  A. Cowey The blindsight saga , 2009, Experimental Brain Research.

[29]  A. Cowey,et al.  The role of light scatter in the residual visual sensitivity of patients with complete cerebral hemispherectomy , 1996, Visual Neuroscience.

[30]  C. Gross,et al.  Afferent basis of visual response properties in area MT of the macaque. II. Effects of superior colliculus removal , 1990, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[31]  C. Gross,et al.  Afferent basis of visual response properties in area MT of the macaque. I. Effects of striate cortex removal , 1989, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[32]  R. Held,et al.  Residual Visual Function after Brain Wounds involving the Central Visual Pathways in Man , 1973, Nature.

[33]  S Zeki,et al.  Conscious visual perception without V1. , 1993, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[34]  Howard S. Hock,et al.  Spatial scale dependent in-phase and anti-phase directional biases in the perception of self-organized motion patterns , 1994, Vision Research.

[35]  K. H. Ruddock,et al.  Visual discrimination of target displacement remains after damage to the striate cortex in humans , 1986, Nature.

[36]  R. W. Kentridge,et al.  Spatial attention speeds discrimination without awareness in blindsight , 2004, Neuropsychologia.

[37]  O. Braddick,et al.  Masking of low frequency information in short-range apparent motion , 1990, Vision Research.

[38]  A. Cowey,et al.  Transneuronal retrograde degeneration of retinal ganglion cells after damage to striate cortex in macaque monkeys: Selective loss of Pβ cells , 1989, Neuroscience.

[39]  T. Sejnowski,et al.  A selection model for motion processing in area MT of primates , 1995, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[40]  L. Weiskrantz,et al.  Parameters affecting conscious versus unconscious visual discrimination with damage to the visual cortex (V1). , 1995, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[41]  Paul Azzopardi,et al.  Is blindsight motion blind , 2001 .

[42]  Bernice E. Rogowitz,et al.  Human Vision and Electronic Imaging II , 1997 .

[43]  Don McNeil,et al.  Epidemiological research methods , 1996 .

[44]  E H Adelson,et al.  Spatiotemporal energy models for the perception of motion. , 1985, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[45]  Paul Schiller,et al.  Stroboskopische Alternativversuche , 1933 .

[46]  K H Ruddock,et al.  Human visual responses in the absence of the geniculo-calcarine projection. , 1980, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[47]  Jean Bullier,et al.  The Role of Area 17 in the Transfer of Information to Extrastriate Visual Cortex , 1994 .

[48]  S. Anstis,et al.  Illusory reversal of visual depth and movement during changes of contrast , 1975, Vision Research.