Longitudinal Relations among Parental Emotional Expressivity and Sympathy and Prosocial Behavior in Adolescence.

Concurrent and longitudinal relations among parental emotional expressivity, children's sympathy, and children's prosocial behavior were assessed with correlations and structural equation modeling when the children were 55 months to 97 months old (n = 214; M age = 73 months, SD = 9.59) and 8 years later (n = 130; ages 150 to 195 months old, M = 171 months, SD = 10.01). Parent emotional expressivity (positive and negative) and children's sympathy were stable across time and early parent-reported sympathy predicted adolescents' sympathy and prosocial behavior. Parents' positive expressivity was positively related to sympathy and prosocial behavior, but in adolescence, this was likely due primarily to consistency over time. Early observed parental negative expressivity was negatively related to adolescents' prosocial behavior. Reported negative expressivity in childhood was negatively related to boys' sympathy in childhood and positively related to girls' sympathy behavior in adolescence. The later relation remained significant when controlling for the stability of parental expressivity and sympathy, suggesting an emerging positive relation between the variables for girls.

[1]  G. S. Hall Adolescence; Its Psychology and Its Relations to Physiology, Anthropology, Sociology, Sex, Crime, Religion and Education , 2012 .

[2]  C. Blair,et al.  Cortisol reactivity is positively related to executive function in preschool children attending head start. , 2005, Child development.

[3]  Nancy Eisenberg,et al.  Prediction of children's empathy-related responding from their effortful control and parents' expressivity. , 2004, Developmental psychology.

[4]  David Warden,et al.  Prosocial children, bullies and victims: An investigation of their sociometric status, empathy and social problem-solving strategies , 2003 .

[5]  J. Youniss,et al.  Two Systems of Youth Service: Determinants of Voluntary and Required Youth Community Service , 2003 .

[6]  Kimberly L. Eaton,et al.  A Meta-Analysis of Family Expressiveness and Children's Emotion Expressiveness and Understanding , 2002 .

[7]  N. Eisenberg,et al.  Mothers' emotional expressivity and children's behavior problems and social competence: mediation through children's regulation. , 2001, Developmental psychology.

[8]  N. Eisenberg,et al.  The relations of parental emotional expressivity with quality of Indonesian children's social functioning. , 2001, Emotion.

[9]  P. Garner,et al.  Emotional Competence, Emotion Socialization, and Young Children's Peer-Related Social Competence , 2001 .

[10]  Philip G. Zimbardo,et al.  Prosocial Foundations of Children's Academic Achievement , 2000, Psychological science.

[11]  M. Hoffman Empathy and moral development : implications for caring and justice , 2000 .

[12]  Robyn Fivush,et al.  Gender Differences in Parent–Child Emotion Narratives , 2000 .

[13]  N. Eisenberg,et al.  Consistency and development of prosocial dispositions: a longitudinal study. , 1999, Child development.

[14]  Tracy L. Spinrad,et al.  The Relations of Parental Affect and Encouragement to Children's Moral Emotions and Behaviour , 1999 .

[15]  Dion C. Buhman,et al.  Facilitating and Disinhibiting Prosocial Behaviors: The Nonlinear Interaction of Trait Perspective Taking and Trait Personal Distress on Volunteering , 1999 .

[16]  N. Eisenberg,et al.  Contemporaneous and Longitudinal Relations of Dispositional Sympathy to Emotionality, Regulation, and Social Functioning , 1999 .

[17]  R. Conger,et al.  Conflict and Cohesion in Parent-Adolescent Relations: Changes in Emotional Expression From Early to Midadolescence , 1998 .

[18]  B. Laursen,et al.  Reconsidering changes in parent-child conflict across adolescence: a meta-analysis. , 1998, Child development.

[19]  Rex B. Kline,et al.  Principles and Practice of Structural Equation Modeling , 1998 .

[20]  J. Gibbs,et al.  Parents' use of inductive discipline: relations to children's empathy and prosocial behavior. , 1996, Child development.

[21]  N. Crick,et al.  The role of overt aggression, relational aggression, and prosocial behavior in the prediction of children's future social adjustment. , 1996, Child development.

[22]  J. Gottman,et al.  Meta-emotion philosophy and family functioning: Reply to Cowan (1996) and Eisenberg (1996). , 1996 .

[23]  John M. Gottman,et al.  Parental meta-emotion philosophy and the emotional life of families: Theoretical models and preliminary data. , 1996 .

[24]  R. Larson,et al.  Changes in adolescents' daily interactions with their families from ages 10 to 18: Disengagement and transformation , 1996 .

[25]  J. Brooks-Gunn,et al.  Transitions and turning points: Navigating the passage from childhood through adolescence. , 1996 .

[26]  P. Bronstein,et al.  Gender and family factors as predictors of late adolescent emotional expressiveness and adjustment: A longitudinal study , 1996 .

[27]  N. Eisenberg,et al.  The role of emotionality and regulation in children's social functioning: a longitudinal study. , 1995, Child development.

[28]  R. Fivush,et al.  Gender differences in parent-child conversations about past emotions: A longitudinal investigation , 1995 .

[29]  R. Parke,et al.  The role of family emotional expressiveness in the development of children's social competence , 1995 .

[30]  N. Fox,et al.  Self-expressiveness within the family context: psychometric support for a new measure , 1995 .

[31]  S. Denham,et al.  Emotional Communication between Mothers and Preschoolers: Relations with Emotional Competence. , 1994 .

[32]  P. Garner,et al.  Social competence among low-income preschoolers: emotion socialization practices and social cognitive correlates. , 1994, Child development.

[33]  B. Laursen,et al.  Interpersonal conflict during adolescence. , 1994, Psychological bulletin.

[34]  M. Eberly,et al.  Unraveling the Ties that Bind: Affective Expression and Perceived Conflict in Parent adolescent Interactions , 1993 .

[35]  M. Browne,et al.  Alternative Ways of Assessing Model Fit , 1992 .

[36]  N. Eisenberg,et al.  The relations of maternal practices and characteristics to children's vicarious emotional responsiveness. , 1992, Child development.

[37]  S. Denham,et al.  Mothers' emotional expressiveness and coping: Relations with preschoolers' social-emotional competence. , 1992 .

[38]  N. Eisenberg,et al.  The relations of parental characteristics and practices to children's vicarious emotional responding. , 1991, Child development.

[39]  D. A. Kenny,et al.  Parental affective style risk and the family system: a social relations model analysis. , 1991, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[40]  N. Eisenberg,et al.  Young Children's Appraisals of Others' Spontaneous Emotional Reactions. , 1991 .

[41]  C. Batson The Altruism Question: Toward A Social-psychological Answer , 1991 .

[42]  T. Achenbach Integrative Guide for the 1991 CBCL/4-18, Ysr, and Trf Profiles , 1991 .

[43]  R. Larson,et al.  Daily emotional states as reported by children and adolescents. , 1989, Child development.

[44]  L. Steinberg Impact of puberty on family relations: Effects of pubertal status and pubertal timing. , 1987 .

[45]  S. Crockenberg Toddlers' Reaction to Maternal Anger. , 1985 .

[46]  S. Crockenberg Toddler's reactions to maternal anger , 1984 .

[47]  A. Blasi,et al.  Bridging Moral Cognition and Moral Action: A Critical Review of the Literature. , 1980 .

[48]  V. Crandall,et al.  A CHILDREN'S SOCIAL DESIRABILITY QUESTIONNAIRE. , 1965, Journal of consulting psychology.

[49]  Tracy L. Spinrad,et al.  The relations of problem behavior status to children's negative emotionality, effortful control, and impulsivity: concurrent relations and prediction of change. , 2005, Developmental psychology.

[50]  M. Nair Parenting adolescents. , 2004, Indian Pediatrics.

[51]  Qing Zhou,et al.  Prosocial development in early adulthood: a longitudinal study. , 2002, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[52]  C. Zahn-Waxler,et al.  Empathy and prosocial patterns in young MZ and DZ twins: Development and genetic and environmental influences. , 2001 .

[53]  J. Hewitt,et al.  Infancy to early childhood : genetic and environmental influences on developmental change , 2001 .

[54]  Kimberly L. Eaton,et al.  Family expressiveness: A retrospective and new directions for research. , 1999 .

[55]  Tracy L. Spinrad,et al.  Parental Socialization of Emotion. , 1998, Psychological inquiry.

[56]  H. Grotevant Adolescent development in family contexts. , 1998 .

[57]  S. West,et al.  The robustness of test statistics to nonnormality and specification error in confirmatory factor analysis. , 1996 .

[58]  G. Trommsdorff Person-Context Relations as Developmental Conditions for Empathy and Prosocial Action: A Cross-Cultural Analysis , 1995 .

[59]  N. Eisenberg,et al.  The Role of Emotionality and Regulation in Children's Social Functioning: A Longitudinal Study , 1995 .

[60]  James L. Carson,et al.  Familial contribution to peer competence among young children: The role of interactive and affective processes , 1992 .

[61]  T. Dix,et al.  The affective organization of parenting: adaptive and maladaptive processes. , 1991, Psychological bulletin.

[62]  Nancy Eisenberg,et al.  Prosocial development in adolescence : a longitudinal study , 1991 .

[63]  Marc H. Bornstein,et al.  Social, emotional, and personality development , 1988 .

[64]  J. P. Rushton,et al.  Extraversion, neuroticism, psychoticism and self-reported delinquency: Evidence from eight separate samples , 1981 .

[65]  J. P. Rushton,et al.  The altruistic personality and the self-report altruism scale , 1981 .

[66]  G. Klumpner,et al.  A pagination converter relating the Gesammelte Werke to the Standard Edition of the complete psychological works of SigMund Freud. , 1971, The International journal of psycho-analysis.