A Psychometric Experiment in Causal Inference to Estimate Evidential Weights Used by Epidemiologists

A psychometric experiment in causal inference was performed on 159 Australian and New Zealand epidemiologists. Subjects each decided whether to attribute causality to 12 summaries of evidence concerning a disease and a chemical exposure. The 1,748 unique summaries embodied predetermined distributions of 19 characteristics generated by computerized evidence simulation. Effects of characteristics of evidence on causal attribution were estimated from logistic regression, and interactions were identified from a regression tree analysis. Factors with the strongest influence on the odds of causal attribution were statistical significance (odds ratio = 4.5 if 0.001 ≤P < 0.05 and 7.2 if P < 0.001, vs P ≥ 0.05); refutation of alternative explanations (odds ratio = 8.1 for no known confounder vs none adjusted); strength of association (odds ratio = 2.0 if 1.5 < relative risk ≤ 2.0 and 3.6 if relative risk > 2.0, vs relative risk ≤ 1.5); and adjunct information concerning biological, factual, and theoretical coherence. The refutation of confounding reduced the cutpoint in the regression tree for decision-making based on strength of association. The effect of the number of supportive studies reached saturation after it exceeded 12 studies. There was evidence of flawed logic in the responses concerning specificity of effects of exposure and a tendency to discount evidence if the P-value was a “near miss” (0.050

[1]  P. Hoffman The paramorphic representation of clinical judgment. , 1960, Psychological bulletin.

[2]  D. Weed,et al.  The practice of causal inference in cancer epidemiology. , 1996, Cancer epidemiology, biomarkers & prevention : a publication of the American Association for Cancer Research, cosponsored by the American Society of Preventive Oncology.

[3]  A. Dickinson,et al.  Within Compound Associations Mediate the Retrospective Revaluation of Causality Judgements , 1996, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. B, Comparative and physiological psychology.

[4]  W. Ahn,et al.  Causal Attribution as a Search for Underlying Mechanisms: An Explanation of the Conjunction Fallacy and the Discounting Principle , 1996, Cognitive Psychology.

[5]  J. Kurinczuk,et al.  The quantification of drug caused morbidity and mortality in Australia 1995 , 1995 .

[6]  J. Hinde,et al.  Models for diagnosing chest pain: is CART helpful? , 1997, Statistics in medicine.

[7]  Charles G. Martin,et al.  Influence of data display formats on physician investigators' decisions to stop clinical trials: prospective trial with repeated measures , 1999, BMJ.

[8]  S Greenland,et al.  Probability Logic and Probabilistic Induction , 1998, Epidemiology.

[9]  C D Naylor,et al.  Measured Enthusiasm: Does the Method of Reporting Trial Results Alter Perceptions of Therapeutic Effectiveness? , 1992, Annals of Internal Medicine.

[10]  P R Yarnold,et al.  Predicting in-hospital mortality of patients with AIDS-related Pneumocystis carinii pneumonia: an example of hierarchically optimal classification tree analysis. , 1997, Statistics in medicine.

[11]  D. Weed,et al.  Biologic plausibility in causal inference: current method and practice. , 1998, American journal of epidemiology.

[12]  L. R. Goldberg Simple models or simple processes? Some research on clinical judgments. , 1968, The American psychologist.

[13]  V. Chinchilli,et al.  Induced Abortion and Risk for Breast Cancer: Reporting (Recall) Bias in a Dutch Case-Control Study , 1996 .

[14]  P. Royston,et al.  Regression using fractional polynomials of continuous covariates: parsimonious parametric modelling. , 1994 .

[15]  K. R. Hammond,et al.  ANALYZING THE COMPONENTS OF CLINICAL INFERENCE. , 1964, Psychological review.

[16]  F. V. van Leeuwen,et al.  Induced abortion and risk for breast cancer: reporting (recall) bias in a Dutch case-control study. , 1996, Journal of the National Cancer Institute.

[17]  J. Richard Landis,et al.  Large sample variance of kappa in the case of different sets of raters. , 1979 .

[18]  M. J. Quadrel,et al.  Risk perception and communication , 2008 .

[19]  B. Kramer,et al.  Induced abortion, bias, and breast cancer: why epidemiology hasn't reached its limit. , 1996, Journal of the National Cancer Institute.

[20]  M. Maclure Demonstration of deductive meta-analysis: ethanol intake and risk of myocardial infarction. , 1993, Epidemiologic reviews.

[21]  R. Dersimonian,et al.  Vasectomy and prostate cancer risk: methodological review of the evidence. , 1993, Journal of clinical epidemiology.

[22]  S Greenland,et al.  Induction versus Popper: substance versus semantics. , 1998, International journal of epidemiology.

[23]  D. Weed,et al.  On the use of causal criteria. , 1997, International journal of epidemiology.

[24]  M. Susser Judgement and causal inference: criteria in epidemiologic studies. , 1977, American journal of epidemiology.

[25]  A. B. Hill The Environment and Disease: Association or Causation? , 1965, Proceedings of the Royal Society of Medicine.